To install click the Add extension button. That's it.

The source code for the WIKI 2 extension is being checked by specialists of the Mozilla Foundation, Google, and Apple. You could also do it yourself at any point in time.

4,5
Kelly Slayton
Congratulations on this excellent venture… what a great idea!
Alexander Grigorievskiy
I use WIKI 2 every day and almost forgot how the original Wikipedia looks like.
Live Statistics
English Articles
Improved in 24 Hours
Added in 24 Hours
Languages
Recent
Show all languages
What we do. Every page goes through several hundred of perfecting techniques; in live mode. Quite the same Wikipedia. Just better.
.
Leo
Newton
Brights
Milds

List of optical illusions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is a list of visual illusions.

Name Example Notes
Afterimage illusion
An afterimage or ghost image is a visual illusion that refers to an image continuing to appear in one's vision after the exposure to the original image has ceased.
Afterimage on empty shape (also known as color dove illusion)
This type of illusions is designed to exploit graphical similarities.
Ambiguous image
These are images that can form two separate pictures. For example, the image shown forms a rabbit and a duck.
Ambigram
A calligraphic design that has multiple or symmetric interpretations.
Ames room illusion
An Ames room is a distorted room that is used to create a visual illusion.
Ames trapezoid window illusion
A window is formed in the shape of a trapezium. It is often hung and spun around to provide the illusion that the window rotates through less than 180 degrees.
Autokinetic effect The autokinetic effect, or autokinesis, occurs when a stationary image appears to move.
Autostereogram

An autostereogram is a single-image stereogram (SIS), designed to create the visual illusion of a three-dimensional (3D) scene from a two-dimensional image in the human brain. An ASCII stereogram is an image that is formed using characters on a keyboard. Magic Eye is an autostereogram book series.
Barberpole illusion
The barber pole illusion is a visual illusion that reveals biases in the processing of visual motion in the human brain.
Benham's top
When a disk that has lines or colours on it is spun, it can form arcs of colour.
Beta movement
Movement that appears to occur when fixed pictures turn on and off.
Bezold Effect
An apparent change of tone of a colour due to the alteration of the colour of the background.
Blivet
Also known as "poiuyt" or "devil's fork", this illusion is an impossible image because in reality the shape cannot exist.
Café wall illusion
This illusion is a pattern in which the mortar or grout between different coloured bricks or tiles on a wall appear to form non-parallel, straight lines, despite the lines being parallel (and straight). Its name comes from a café wall that produced the illusion.
Catoptric cistula
A catoptric cistula is a box with insides made of mirrors so as to distort images of objects put into the box.
Checker shadow illusion

The checker shadow illusion shows that when a shadow is cast onto a checked board, the colours of squares A and B in the photos appear to be different, when in fact they are the same.
Chubb illusion
The Chubb illusion is an optical illusion or error in visual perception in which the apparent contrast of an object varies substantially to most viewers depending on its relative contrast to the field on which it is displayed.
Color constancy
Colour constancy is an example of subjective constancy and a feature of the human color perception system which ensures that the perceived color of objects remains relatively constant under varying illumination conditions. A green apple for instance looks green to us at midday, when the main illumination is white sunlight, and also at sunset, when the main illumination is red.
Color phi phenomenon The color phi phenomenon is a perceptual illusion in which a disembodied perception of motion is produced by a succession of still images.
Contingent perceptual aftereffect
Convergence micropsia
Cornsweet illusion
An illusion where two colours can obviously be seen to be different when placed directly beside each other; however, when the two colours are separated by a thick black line, they appear to be of the same hue.
Crater illusion
A type of multistable illusion where an image of a concave object, rotated so that the light source is below, may sometimes appear convex, and vice versa. This phenomenon is due to the fact that light sources tend to shine from above the subject.
Delboeuf illusion
An optical illusion of relative size perception. The two black circles are exactly the same size; however, the one on the left seems larger.
Disappearing Model A trompe-l'œil body painting by Joanne Gair.
The dress
Two ways in which the photograph of The dress may be perceived: blue and black under a yellow-tinted illumination (left figure) or white and gold under a blue-tinted illumination (right figure).
An optical illusion resulting from the brain's attempt to discount coloured tinting from daylight and other sources.[1] The dress was a viral phenomenon, which was either seen as blue and black or as white and gold. Its true colours were eventually confirmed to be blue and black.[2]
Ebbinghaus illusion
The Ebbinghaus illusion, or Titchener circles, is an optical illusion of relative size perception. The two orange circles are exactly the same size; however, the one on the right appears larger.
Ehrenstein illusion

The Ehrenstein illusion is an optical illusion studied by the German psychologist Walter Ehrenstein in which the sides of a square placed inside a pattern of concentric circles take an apparent curved shape.
Fata Morgana (mirage)
Visible in a narrow band right above the horizon, Fata Morgana mirages significantly distort the object or objects on which they are based, often such that the object is completely unrecognizable. A Fata Morgana may be seen on land or at sea, in polar regions, or in deserts. It may involve almost any kind of distant object, including boats, islands, and the coastline.
Fechner color
Figure-ground (perception)
Filling-in
Flash lag illusion
Forced perspective
Application used in film and architecture to create the illusion of larger, more distant objects.
Fraser spiral illusion
The Fraser spiral illusion, or false spiral, or the twisted cord illusion, was first described by the British psychologist Sir James Fraser in 1908. The overlapping black arc segments appear to form a spiral; however, the arcs are a series of concentric circles.
Gravity hill
Grid illusion

Any kind of grid that deceives a person's vision. The two most common types of grid illusions are the Hermann grid illusion (1870) and the scintillating grid illusion (1994). The first is characterized by "ghostlike" grey blobs perceived at the intersections of a white (or light-colored) grid on a black background. The grey blobs disappear when looking directly at an intersection. The second is constructed by superimposing white discs on the intersections of orthogonal gray bars on a black background. Dark dots seem to appear and disappear rapidly at random intersections, hence the label "scintillating". When a person keeps their eyes directly on a single intersection, the dark dot does not appear. The dark dots disappear if one is too close to or too far from the image.
Hering illusion
The Hering illusion (1861): When two straight and parallel lines are presented in front of radial background (like the spokes of a bicycle), the lines appear as if they were bowed outwards.
Hollow-Face illusion
The Hollow-Face illusion is an optical illusion in which the perception of a concave mask of a face appears as a normal convex face.
Hybrid image
A Hybrid image is an optical illusion developed at MIT in which an image can be interpreted in one of two different ways depending on viewing distance.
Illusory contours
Illusory contours or subjective contours are a form of visual illusion where contours are perceived without a luminance or color change across the contour.
Impossible object
Irradiation illusion
Isometric illusion
An isometric illusion (also called an ambiguous figure or inside/outside illusion) is a type of optical illusion, specifically one due to multistable perception.
Jastrow illusion
The Jastrow illusion is an optical illusion discovered by the American psychologist Joseph Jastrow in 1889.
Kanizsa triangle
The Kanizsa triangle is an optical illusion first described by the Italian psychologist Gaetano Kanizsa in 1955. It is a triangle formed of illusory contours.
Kinetic Depth Effect The Kinetic depth effect refers to the phenomenon whereby the three-dimensional structural form of a silhouette can be perceived when the object is moving. In the absence of other visual depth cues, this might be the only perception mechanism available to infer the object's shape. Additionally the direction of motion can reverse due to the existence of multiple 3D visual solutions.
Leaning tower illusion The Leaning tower illusion is an optical illusion that presents two identical images of the Leaning Tower of Pisa side by side.
Lilac chaser
Lilac chaser is a visual illusion, also known as the Pac-Man illusion.
Liquid crystal shutter glasses
Lunar terminator illusion Lunar terminator illusion is an optical illusion where the apparent source of sunlight illuminating the moon does not corresponding with the actual position of the sun.
Mach bands
Mach bands is an optical illusion named after the physicist Ernst Mach.
McCollough effect
The McCollough effect (1965) is a phenomenon of human visual perception in which colorless gratings appear colored contingent on the orientation of the gratings. It is an aftereffect requiring a period of induction to produce it.
Missing square puzzle

The missing square puzzle is an optical illusion used in mathematics classes to help students reason about geometrical figures.
Moon illusion
The Moon illusion is an optical illusion in which the Moon appears larger near the horizon than it does while higher up in the sky.
Motion aftereffect
Motion illusion
Müller-Lyer illusion
The Müller-Lyer illusion is an optical illusion consisting of a stylized arrow.
Multistability
Musion Eyeliner
Necker cube
The Necker cube is an optical illusion first published in 1832 by Swiss crystallographer Louis Albert Necker.
Numerosity adaptation effect
Orbison illusion
The Orbison illusion is an optical illusion that was first described by the psychologist William Orbison in 1939.
Oppel-Kundt illusion The Oppel-Kundt illusion is an optical illusion named after German physicists Johann Joseph Oppel [de] (first mentioned this phenomenon in 1860) and August Kundt (first performed a systematic study of the illusion in 1863).
Penrose stairs
The Penrose stairs was created by Lionel Penrose and his son Roger Penrose.[3] A variation on the Penrose triangle, it is a two-dimensional depiction of a staircase in which the stairs make four 90-degree turns as they ascend or descend yet form a continuous loop, so that a person could climb them forever and never get any higher.
Penrose triangle
The Penrose triangle was first created by the Swedish artist Oscar Reutersvärd in 1934. The mathematician Roger Penrose independently devised and popularised it in the 1950s, describing it as "impossibility in its purest form".
Pepper's ghost
Perceived visual angle
Peripheral drift illusion
A motion illusion (1979/1999) generated by the presentation of a sawtooth luminance grating in the visual periphery.
Phantogram
Phantograms, also known as Phantaglyphs, Op-Ups, free-standing anaglyphs, levitated images, and book anaglyphs, are a form of optical illusion.
Phi phenomenon
Poggendorff illusion
The Poggendorff illusion (1860) involves the misperception of the position of one segment of a transverse line that has been interrupted by the contour of an intervening structure (here a rectangle).
Ponzo illusion
In the Ponzo illusion (1911) two identical lines across a pair of converging lines, similar to railway tracks, are drawn. The upper line looks longer because we interpret the converging sides according to linear perspective as parallel lines receding into the distance. In this context, we interpret the upper line as though it were farther away, so we see it as longer – a farther object would have to be longer than a nearer one for both to produce retinal images of the same size.
Pulfrich effect The Pulfrich effect is the effect that covering one eye with transparent but darkened glass can cause purely lateral motion to appear to have a depth component even though in reality it doesn't; even a completely flat scene such as one shown on a television screen can appear to exhibit some three-dimensional motion, but this is an illusion due to the fact that darkening the scene for one eye causes the photoreceptors in that eye to respond more slowly.
Rubin vase
Rubin vase (1915): an ambiguous or bi-stable (i.e., reversing) two-dimensional form.
Sander illusion
In Sander's parallelogram (1926) the diagonal line bisecting the larger, left-hand parallelogram appears to be considerably longer than the diagonal line bisecting the smaller, right-hand parallelogram, but is in fact the same length.
Silencing
Silencing is an illusion in which a set of objects that change in luminance, hue, size, or shape appears to stop changing when it moves.
Size–weight illusion The size–weight illusion is also known as the Charpentier illusion or Charpentier–Koseleff illusion.
Stepping feet illusion
The stepping feet illusion is influenced by the contrast between moving objects and their background.
Stroboscopic effect
Swept-plane display
Ternus illusion The Ternus illusion (1926/1938) is based upon apparent motion.
Thaumatrope
A thaumatrope is a toy that was popular in Victorian times.
Trompe-l'œil
Troxler's fading Troxler's fading: When one fixates on a particular point for even a short period of time, an unchanging stimulus away from the fixation point will fade away and disappear.
Vanishing puzzle
A vanishing puzzle is a mechanical optical illusion showing different numbers of a certain object when parts of the puzzle are moved around.[4]
Vertical–horizontal illusion
The Vertical-horizontal illusion is the tendency for observers to overestimate the length of a vertical line relative to a horizontal line of the same length.
Visual tilt effects

Wagon-wheel effect
White's illusion
Wundt illusion
The two red vertical lines are both straight, but they may look as if they are bowed inwards to some observers. The distortion is induced by the crooked lines on the background
Zoetrope
Zöllner illusion
The Zöllner illusion is a classic optical illusion named after its discoverer, German astrophysicist Johann Karl Friedrich Zöllner.

YouTube Encyclopedic

  • 1/5
    Views:
    27 856
    47 809
    60 003
    2 655 873
    62 184
  • 25 Mind Blowing Optical Illusions That Play Tricks on Your Brain
  • 25 CRAZY Optical Illusions That Will Blow Your Mind
  • 25 Insane Optical Illusions That Will Leave You Dazed And Confused
  • 10 OPTICAL ILLUSIONS That Will Blow Your Mind | LIST KING
  • 25 Optical Illusions And Phenomena Only Seen In Nature

Transcription

See also

Notes

  1. ^ "The Science of Why No One Agrees on the Colour of This Dress". Wired. 2015-02-27. Archived from the original on 28 February 2015. Retrieved 27 February 2015.
  2. ^ "Optical illusion: Dress color debate goes global". BBC News. BBC. Archived from the original on 2019-07-13. Retrieved 28 February 2015.
  3. ^ Penrose, LS; Penrose, R. (1958). "Impossible objects: A special type of optical illusion". British Journal of Psychology. 49 (1): 31–33. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1958.tb00634.x. PMID 13536303.
  4. ^ The Guardian, Vanishing Leprechaun, Disappearing Dwarf and Swinging Sixties Pin-up Girls – puzzles in pictures

External links

This page was last edited on 27 June 2024, at 08:44
Basis of this page is in Wikipedia. Text is available under the CC BY-SA 3.0 Unported License. Non-text media are available under their specified licenses. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc. WIKI 2 is an independent company and has no affiliation with Wikimedia Foundation.