To install click the Add extension button. That's it.

The source code for the WIKI 2 extension is being checked by specialists of the Mozilla Foundation, Google, and Apple. You could also do it yourself at any point in time.

4,5
Kelly Slayton
Congratulations on this excellent venture… what a great idea!
Alexander Grigorievskiy
I use WIKI 2 every day and almost forgot how the original Wikipedia looks like.
Live Statistics
English Articles
Improved in 24 Hours
Added in 24 Hours
What we do. Every page goes through several hundred of perfecting techniques; in live mode. Quite the same Wikipedia. Just better.
.
Leo
Newton
Brights
Milds

Retrograde amnesia

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Retrograde amnesia
SpecialtyNeurology

In neurology, retrograde amnesia (RA) is the inability to access memories or information from before an injury or disease occurred.[1] RA differs from a similar condition called anterograde amnesia (AA), which is the inability to form new memories following injury or disease onset.[2] Although an individual can have both RA and AA at the same time, RA can also occur on its own; this 'pure' form of RA can be further divided into three types: focal, isolated, and pure RA.[3] RA negatively affects an individual's episodic, autobiographical, and declarative memory, but they can still form new memories because RA leaves procedural memory intact.[3] Depending on its severity, RA can result in either temporally graded or more permanent memory loss.[3] However, memory loss usually follows Ribot's law, which states that individuals are more likely to lose recent memories than older memories.[4] Diagnosing RA generally requires using an Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI) and observing brain structure through magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), a computed tomography scan (CT), or electroencephalography (EEG).[3][5][6]

YouTube Encyclopedic

  • 1/5
    Views:
    72 266
    1 711
    991
    6 496
    497
  • Lec 11 | MIT 9.00SC Introduction to Psychology, Spring 2011
  • Exceptional memory
  • Bergson's Holographic Theory - 16 - Storing Experience (Not) in the Brain
  • Episodic Memory, Time, and Agency: Some Constraints from Neuropsychology
  • Indications for CT scan after minor head trauma

Transcription

The following content is provided under a Creative Commons license. Your support will help MIT OpenCourseWare continue to offer high quality educational resources for free. To make a donation or view additional materials from hundreds of MIT courses, visit MIT OpenCourseWare at ocw.mit.edu. PROFESSOR: So when we talked about learning, we said that everything we know comes from learning except that which is given to us by our genes. Our knowledge of how to walk around in the world, to read, to think, to talk, to feel, the values that we have, the hopes we have, the fears we have, they're all learned through experience. And then we talked about another approach to thinking about how we learn things through memory, and today I'm going to talk about what we understand to be the brain organization of human memory. And Oliver Sacks, in The Lost Mariner, chapter 2, talks about one of the two major kinds of amnesia, the loss of memory due to a brain disorder or a brain injury. But he has a very nice language from Luis Bunuel talking about memory. "You have begun to lose your memory, if only in bits and pieces, to realize that memory is what makes our life. Life without memory is no life at all. Our memories, our coherence, our reason, our feeling, even our action. Without it, we are nothing. I can only wait for the final amnesia, the one that erases an entire life as it did my mother's." Right? So our memories of our lives, what we've done, where we've been, what mattered to us-- that's a huge piece of who we are. And he runs into a patient who came into the hospital early in 1975 with a note saying "Helpless, demented, confused, and disoriented." He had what we call clinically Korsakoff's Syndrome. So these are individuals who have severe alcoholism for many years, and a subset of those people develop a severe memory disorder, like this man. He's 49 years old. He tells Sacks about his life, details of his life, going into World War II, that he worked on a submarine, and so on. And Sacks says, "a full and interesting life remembered visually in detail, but for some reason his memories stop around World War II." Then he asked him, what year is it? Remember the year is 1975. And he says, it's 1945. What do you mean? We've won the war, FDR-- Franklin Delano Roosevelt-- is dead, Truman's at the helm. These are great times ahead. And Jimmy, how old would you be? He stopped and he said, well, I guess I'm 19. That would have been his age, you know, 20 years ago, right? 30 years ago. So he's stuck in time. He thinks it's historically a while ago. He has the completely wrong age. And Sacks takes out a mirror and throws it at him and says, what do you-- what happened? And he says, Jesus Christ, what's going on? What happened? Is this a nightmare? Am I crazy? Is this a joke? So it's as if he's lost the last 30 years of his life. And he's living as if it were 30 years ago. And then when he walks back in, he's hardly recognized. Sacks is hardly recognized by the person. On intelligence, he shows excellent ability, but his memory-- very quickly, he forgets things. What is this, I asked him, showing him a photo in a magazine I was holding. It's the moon. No it's not, if it's a picture of the Earth taken from the moon. I'm mean, don't forget, in 1945, nobody imagined that we would be sending up people to the moon, right? And taking pictures from the moon of the Earth. You're used to it. It's nothing to you. No it's not. It's a picture of the Earth taken from the moon. Doc, you're kidding. Somebody would have to get a camera up there. Naturally. Hell, you're joking. How would you know that? So he has lost the last 30 years of his life, for all practical purposes, despite normal intelligence and normal thought. So we're going to talk more about how this occurs in another patient, and more generally, how the brain supports your ability to learn things. So here's this remarkable brain. And in one sense, you could say it's almost all there to absorb things in life. You have very small insects that can do a lot with the genes they're given. We have big brains to learn the life we lead. And I'm going to come back at the end to this metaphor, but the idea that in many ways, your brain as you sit there right now, or any healthy brain, is kind of like a symphony orchestra that has many specialized instruments for learning different kinds of things. And you feel like a unified whole, and you are, and these instruments interact with each other. But just like a symphony orchestra, you know, together they make a sound. So imagine you're a Martian who lands outside a symphony hall, and you hear a symphony playing. It's something beautiful. It sounds really interesting. If you're a Martian on the outside of the symphony hall, how many instruments are you guessing, how many things might you guess, are making that sound? Would you have any idea? One big one? Two big ones? 50? It'd be very hard to guess. And it's only been in the last 20 years or so that we had some real clarification on the instruments of learning in our brains. So I'm going to talk about three things today. Anterograde amnesia, the loss of the ability to form new memories; retrograde amnesia, the loss of memories you already had; and that idea of memory systems, this idea that each of us has a symphony of learning instruments in our brain that empowers us to learn different kinds of things. So anterograde amnesia is, again, the inability to remember new information. And people mostly talk about two kinds, but as you see, they're most of life, OK? Not all of that-- we'll talk about that-- but a lot of it, such as events you experience or facts you encounter. So think about this. All the events you've remembered from your entire life, all the facts you ever learned in school, at home, on the computer-- that's a huge amount of what you know. And a patient like we just described or the patients I'll describe in a moment lose the ability to remember any event from the day they become amnesic or remember much in the way of new information of any kind, in terms of facts of the world. So in what sense is memory in the brain distributed or localized? In what sense is it in one place or many places? And this has been a central debate in all of neuroscience, but in memory, too. And Karl Lashley, a giant of neuroscience, who worked just two T stops away at Harvard, said this: "It is not possible to demonstrate the isolated localization of a memory trace anywhere in the nervous system. The engram is represented throughout the brain." The engram's kind of a magic word in memory and the brain. The engram is the thing in you that changes in your brain that is the stuff of a memory. If you learn today that there's more terrible news in Japan with the nuclear reactors, and you remember that because it matters to you, because you care about the people, there's a physical change in your brain that is the memory. If you remember what I'm saying now for more than a few moments, that is a physical change in your brain. So all of us in neuroscience are amazed by that. How can we physically change the brain to be the biological record of a memory, that we can keep and use in the future? And the engram, Karl Lashley said, is represented throughout the brain. It's spread throughout the brain. Here's why he thought that. So he was working with rats, and they ran a maze. And you saw how quickly you learned the maze to go from start to finish to get the food if you're a rat. And here's what he found, and the results won't amaze you, to start with. So what he did is he made-- this is number of errors during learning, so it's good to be low. And this is how much neocortex they took out, ranging from 10% to 80%. And he discovered that the more neocortex you took out, the worse learner you were. Now if it didn't happen in some broad sense, we'd say, well, what part of us is learning it? You take out more brain, you're less of a good reader. 10% out, 10% percent bad memory. 50% of brain out, 50% of bad memory. 80% of the brain out, 80% bad memory, right? OK, that makes sense. But what he was really impressed by is it didn't seem to him to matter where he took the brain out of the rat. It just didn't matter. It's just the percent that he took out. Whatever percent he took out is how bad the learner you were. And we'll come back to that, but that's why I said, it must be spread everywhere, because wherever I take out brain, part of the memory is weakened or disappears. And sometimes people call that mass action for distributed memory, that's spread throughout the brain in this mass way. It's just spread all over the place. And a single clinical case-- I'll spend a few moments on him for two reasons. It is the historical case that turned this around, and I have a particular perspective on this because when I was a graduate student, I did a lot of work with this single patient whose case turned around our understanding of the mammalian brain and memory. So as I start to talk about it-- I can't tell you what Phineas Gage was really like, beyond what I read, OK? Or Broca's patients from the 1900s. I can tell you what HM, the man we're about to talk about, was like because I spent many, many hours with him, two blocks from here, when I was a graduate student in Suzanne Corkin's lab. So the structure we'll focus on is something called the hippocampus. It looks sort of big in the picture. It's about the size of about 2/3 of your thumb, one on the left and one on the right in the insides of the medial part of the temporal lobes. A pretty small percentage of the brain. And you can see it's located here. Here it is sort of drawn out. And here's the story of this man. He died a couple years ago. There's lots of plays and movies and books in the works. I can also tell you that when I was a graduate student here, my fellow graduate students would say, tell us who HM, what his initials really stand for, as if it really mattered, particularly. But I felt privileged to have the secret knowledge of what his name was. When he died, they published it. Everybody knows now, so my secret is out. Or our secret is out. He was born in 1926. At age 16 he had his first major epileptic seizure. By age 27, he was having many, many seizures a day. You can have petit mal seizures, where you tune out from the world. You can have grand mal seizures that lead to physical convulsions. By age 27, the medications were not helping him, in 1953, very much, that were available. And he was just sitting in his house just waiting for the next seizure, basically. That was his life. He was a man of normal intelligence and fairly typical, average background. Nothing remarkable. And in order to treat the epilepsy, William Scoville, the surgeon, resected, or took out surgically, the tissue. Here's this cartoon of what he took out-- the hippocampus, also the amygdala just in front of it, also tissue that surrounds it. And the reason he took it out is the single most likely place where epileptic seizures start is the hippocampus. It's kind of a signal, in many ways, that the hippocampus does something very dangerous. It's operating in some sort of fast speed lane of neurobiology. It's a very sensitive part of the brain, for lack of oxygen, for diseases. We'll talk about Alzheimer's today. It's a very vulnerable part of the brain, but it does something magical and essential for memory. And so they couldn't locate where the seizures were starting, very well, from him in ways they typically do, so they said, OK. He has all these seizures. We don't know what to do. Let's take out the left and the right hippocampus and the surrounding tissue. And in terms of seizure control, it was pretty successful. He had to stay on medications from that day 'til a few years ago, when he passed away. But he had very, very few seizures compared to before that. So it was medically very successful in terms of seizure control. It had a giant side effect. From that day 'til a couple years ago, HM never formed a new memory for any event, or for all practical purposes, for any fact that he was exposed to ever again, despite his normal intelligence and his otherwise intact abilities. So they took an MR of HM. It took a long time to decide they could even take a MR picture of HM, because they were very worried that if you put him in a MR scanner, there's magnetic fields, and often they put in clips after surgery. Regularly they put in clips after surgery, to keep the vessels from bleeding. And they weren't sure what the clips were made of for a while. And so they said, well, we can't put him in, because we could move the clips and kill him, which would be bad. So they thought it was dangerous. And they did a huge detective process of going to factory that made them and talking to people who built it before they decided-- it took, like, 10 years of research to decide that the clips were not metal and they could take an MR. And even here-- here's the top of the brain, normal person. Here's the hippocampus, the small structure in you-- pretty small. It's removed in him, but you can see he's got a lot of brain left, except for these removed structures. But what happened was he had such a fantastic impairment of learning new information, it got the label of a global amnesia. Global amnesia. It didn't matter whether tests were hard, like remembering stuff or multiple choice. He failed practically every standard test of memory you'd ever give a person. Or if it's words or nonsense syllables, faces, clicks, mazes, public events, assassinations of presidents, wars, going to the moon, whatever you think of as famous-- he didn't know they happened at all. Personal events. The death of his own parents. If you asked him how his parents were doing, he would say something like, I haven't seen them for a while. He would not know they had passed away. So no matter how publicly famous something was, no matter how personally important it was, that knowledge-- he could remember it for a few seconds, and we'll talk about that more, but it would drift away completely within seconds. No matter how often he heard it, no matter how personally important it was. And you could say, what does he say his experience is like? So Brenda Milner, the neuropsychologist, who did a lot of the critical original research with him, describing his case said that he's said, "Every day is alone in itself, whatever joy I've had and whatever sorrow I've had." I mean, it sounds very poetic, and it's true, except that for HM, his memory lasts seconds. So he doesn't remember any day since 1950 or something. So I think his experience is more like this. "Right now I'm wondering, Have I done or said anything amiss? You see, at this moment, everything looks clear to me." His mind is clear. He's smart. "But what happened just before?" What happened a minute ago, or two minutes ago, or five minutes ago? "That's what worries me. It's like waking from a dream; I just don't remember." All right? So when we worked with him here, a few blocks from here, he wouldn't know who I was. He wouldn't know why he was here. He was an extremely pleasant and wonderful research participant. A piece of that was-- he was very nice and very pleasant. A piece of that was he didn't know time was passing. So if any of you are involved in experiments as participants, sometimes people enjoy the experiment for about five minutes but not for the next 55, because they're getting a lot of measurements to get a good measurement. For HM, it was never an hour-long experiment. It was always a minute and a half experiment. So you had to be very careful not to abuse that situation as a researcher. So here's his name, Henry Molaison. Here's his pictures from his youth and from this. But you see how he had a complete reversal? All of a sudden, the removal just two small regions-- the left and the right hippocampus in the brain-- stopped the creation, of formation of new memories altogether and in any practical way. So that shows you that memory's not spread throughout the brain in a sort of undifferentiated mass-action way, but that particular parts of the brain play very particular roles in how memories are formed. And the hippocampus plays a huge role in that. Can't see it. So they had a big project where they brought him from the nursing home in Connecticut where he was. They drove him by ambulance to Massachusetts General Hospital. They overnight did a something like an eight hour MRI scan on him, to have the world's most detailed MRI scan you practically have on anybody. And then they removed his brain and sent it to a laboratory in San Diego, where they took his brain and they sliced it into many, many, many incredibly thin slices that they're going to post on the web. And on the web for quite a while was the slicing of his brain. He consented to this, all this research effort, and finally his brain is a gift to research and understanding the brain and memory. So let me tell you a couple of experimental results from him, and then you might think of questions you have about what a man like this is like, OK? So we talked about last time about that if you give a list of words to people, and you try to do it yourself, that people remember the first words best-- the primacy effect, a long-term memory effect that's not affected by adding a few seconds of delay-- and a recency affect-- superior memory for the last few words of the list that's strongly affected by adding in just 10 seconds of delay from the last word until you recall. And this is done with other amnesic patients, and not HM, but a similar disorder. You can see that their memory is worse overall-- there, the dark line-- but they have some benefit from the beginning of the list and-- but it's small. It's not the same as the control-- but they're completely normal, completely normal for the last two or three words. So this is what we mean by impaired long-term memory and spared short-term memory. But the short-term memory's just moments. But it's not just an impression you have. It's a scientific experiment you can do. Here's another-- we did this last time. We said, if you read digits to people and ask them to repeat them back aloud, that we have a limited short-term memory of seven plus or minus two. People get to about seven or eight, typically. So there's a mean test that's given to look at something. So here's what they do. Imagine that you do this test, and you were correct for two digits, correct for three, repeating them back after you heard them. Four, five, six, seven, you did well. And then you made mistakes at eight, which would be very typical for a human. So your span, therefore, is seven. That's how much you can hold in short-term memory. It varies a little bit from person to person. And now they're going to do all the rest of the experiment at one beyond your span. One beyond what you can do. One beyond what your short-term memory-- here's the limit of your short-term memory. Let's add one thing. And one thing that we know helps memory is repetition. It's not the best way to learn things, but it is a powerful way to learn things. So now they give the entire experiment with eight digits at a time for this person, one beyond what you could report. So they give you this, and they give you this. But they give you one boost, which is every third one, it's the same sequence over and over again. So some you hear once and once only. And one, you hear over and over again. Is that OK? Now what would happen if you think, if you hear eight digits over and over again? What would happen? You know this in your own life. Your Social Security number, telephone numbers, things-- what happens? Can you learn eight digits if you do it over and--? Yeah. It wouldn't be easy, but you get it with repetition. So here's what happens in typical people. Here's the repetition. Here's how well they do for the non-repeated super-span ones. They never get that good because they basically can't do that. Occasionally they get lucky, but mostly not. But the ones that repeat, they get better and better. So repetition is another way to break the bounds of short-term memory and register something in long-term memory. But in HM, they did this for hundreds of trials, and he never got better at the repeated series. So you could do endless repetition with him, or it could be personally important. He wouldn't remember it. So let me add one more piece to the story. If I talk to you and say, here are the digits, tell that back to me, that's an auditory-verbal experiment. So they're words, and you're hearing them. You can also test short-term memory and long-term memory using a visuo-spatial test of blocks. So you're seeing what the examiner sees, with numbers on the back of each of these. The person taking the test just sees these blocks as black boxes without any number. You need that because you have a piece of paper with numbers on it that tell you what to tap. OK? So when you give this test, you might see five, seven, eight, and you would tap with your hand, five, seven, eight, and the person would tap that back. And I can tell you what made me pretty nervous when I gave this test to a person who had a long one? Because I'd have to read the numbers, practice it, and I'd be desperately trying to get eight my head, which I could barely do, maybe. Because I could tap eight back, so if a person was-- and then we'd do the same thing. We'd say, if you can do eight, we'll test you at nine and repeat them. And here's two findings, and let's talk about HM for a moment. So one finding is now, in patients now-- we're going to switch one thing a little bit. HM had the left and the right hippocampus removed. In these patients, they had either the left or the right removed. Either the left or the right. So here's what happens for normal controls, for the numbers that are beyond their span. Not too good, low performance, but they benefit from repetition. Here's what happens for the patients with left hippocampal removals for the auditory-verbal things for the digits. So they're good for immediate memory for this stuff, but they're terrible at developing a long-term memory over here, if it's verbal. But the patients with the right temporal ones are just fine. For spatial things, it's exactly the opposite. The group that's really impaired are the patients with a large right hippocampal removal, and the people with the left hippocampal do just fine. So we have two separations in the brain for learning things in the medial temporal lobes. Neither the left nor the right are involved in short-term memory, short-term memory lasting only seconds. The left is important for verbal long-term memories, and the right is important for long-term spatial memories, which is very consistent with what we know about left and right hemispheres for verbal and spatial knowledge. So let me say a word more about how we think about the organization of memory, and we'll come back to this chart. So we're talking here, so far, about what people call declarative or explicit memory. You study some material, you get tested on it. What did you study? And people sometimes make a difference between two kinds of things. So episodic memory might be if I ask you-- OK, I need a volunteer in your seat. It's really easy, this one. OK, thanks. I saw some people getting ready to go. Well, OK. OK, what did you have for breakfast this morning? And you don't have to answer honestly if its embarrassing. You can just give us a standard-- (LAUGHING) I'm not looking to make-- yeah. AUDIENCE: Cereal. PROFESSOR: Cereal. How about you? AUDIENCE: What? AUDIENCE: Yogurt. PROFESSOR: Yogurt. Cereal and yogurt. Two excellent healthy answers. OK. So now I'm going to ask you-- if I could ask you first. How many feet in a yard? AUDIENCE: Three. PROFESSOR: Excellent. OK. And now I'm going to ask you, when and where did you learn that? OK? And you might go, I don't know. I just learned it. Right? Or if I ask you, capital of France. AUDIENCE: Paris. PROFESSOR: Paris. Excellent answer. When was the shocking moment when this was revealed to you? When you said, I'll never trust the world again-- yes. Who knows, right? So what you're learning is, episodic memory is memory for specific events, like what I did last night, what I had for breakfast, specific time and place. Semantic memory is what we could call generic memory. We know lots of things about the world, but we might not know when or where we learned. So that's a distinction psychologists find useful to think about. So how about in HM? We said he can't remember digits, or faces, or things like that. How about generic memory? So here's an experiment that I was involved in, where we said-- because there were some debates about this at the time. So we gave HM a multiple choice test where he saw words or phrases that entered the language after the onset of his amnesia. So we thought he wouldn't learn them, but we didn't know for sure, from just everyday experience. And by the way, I should tell you, he watched tons of television. What his experience was like watching television is an interesting question. You should be thinking now about questions you want to ask about HM. In a moment, I'll come back to that. But look at-- so, he has four choices for the word amniocentesis, including the correct one, but he picks "an infectious inflammatory disease of the intestines." apartheid? In front of him is the correct answer, two other answers, and he picks, "the separation of young cows that have not yet given birth to calves." Boat people? "People who cater bon voyage parties," which is completely the wrong-- Brainwash, "a fluid that surrounds and bathes the brain." Granola, "a portable keyboard wind instrument." Software, "expensive clothing made of a soft, twilled fabric." What's going on with him? Are these, like, crazy weird answers? Or how would you-- what do we think is going on with him? Why is he picking these? The other one is right in front of him. What's he doing? AUDIENCE: Looking at words he knows. PROFESSOR: Yeah. He's doing what teachers teach you in school. Take apart the word into its parts, right? And so he goes, software, soft clothes that you wear. But what does that mean? He's missed, in every sense, the advent of computers and software in the world. The idea, the concept, the words-- it's as if it didn't exist, because in 1953, although software existed, it wasn't a very popular concept. Most people didn't know about the concept of that. In 1953-- the way we looked at these, these were not words that were in the dictionary in the 1950s. So now, what would you like to know about a man like HM? Yeah. AUDIENCE: So obviously he couldn't remember something that happened two hours ago, but if you're having a conversation with him, would he not remember what you guys were talking about? Or could he remember the context of the conversation? PROFESSOR: Right. So you're saying, he wouldn't remember something two days ago, but you're talking with him. What's that like? Right? So let me give you one experimental thing and then an exper-- so one experiment they did is they left him with something like six numbers, well within the span, and said, remember these numbers. And it could be, you know, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6-- well, that wouldn't work so well. 2, 4, 6, 8, 9. 2, 4, 6, 8, 9. They leave the room for 15 minutes. They come back in. He's an excellent subject. He's going, 2, 4, 6, 8, 9. 2, 4, 6, 8, 9. What were the numbers? 2, 4, 6, 8, 9. They go, that's excellent. Was there any trick you did to do that? Or did you just-- And he'd go, oh, oh, oh. What were the numbers? As soon as a thought is out of the forefront of his mind, it's gone. So, yeah. He would not recognize us, and we spent many hours with him. A striking thing, which was amazing at first and then sometimes irksome if you worked with him-- and he was a delightful guy-- was that he would tell you a story from his past. And he didn't have a huge array of stories, because they were all from a long time ago. And he would tell you about a gun collection he had. And he would tell he had three guns, and they had this property and that property. He'd finish the story, and if you just stayed quiet for a moment, he would forget that he told you the story, but it would be vaguely on his mind because he just told it to you. And he would say to you, hey, did I tell you about my gun collection? And he would tell you, almost verbatim, the same story. You'd just wait a moment. He forgot that he told you this story, but it's slightly on his mind because you just told you it. And he goes, hey, did I tell you my-- he could tell you this until you could bear it no longer. So it was kind of amazing how short his short-term memory was. Without this part of the brain, it's just a few seconds. His conversations were OK, but I would call them pretty shallow. I don't know how to put it. You know, when you talk with somebody and you're on the phone, and they're doing something else, and you can tell-- for him it was a little bit like that. He was-- so, good social convention. He would smile. He would chat with you. But if what you were talking about depended on remembering something from three sentences ago in any detail, gone. Just seconds, unless he's practicing and practicing and practicing without doing anything else. Yeah. AUDIENCE: So he did have the capability to learn procedurally. PROFESSOR: Yes. We're going to come to that. So the question is, did he ever learn-- he had other kinds of learning that were amazingly completely normal in him. Procedural memory, you're absolutely right. And we'll come to that just in about 20 minutes, OK? Exactly right. And if I don't answer that well, make me do that again. OK? But yeah. Yeah? AUDIENCE: Was he able to remember that he couldn't remember things? PROFESSOR: Yeah. Really good question. Was he able to remember that he couldn't remember things? Yes, he knew he had a bad memory. So now the question is, in what sense did he know he had a bad memory? Like, I know I can't draw very well, amongst many other things I can't do very well. But if you talked to him, he'd say, yeah, I don't have a good memory. But he's looking at you, and he's going, like, I don't know where I am, who you are, or what I'm doing. Everything is vague, for many, many years. I must have bad memory. Like, you would too, like a science fiction movie. You woke up, you didn't know who you were, where you were, what you were doing, and you can't remember anything except from the most distant past. You'd go, something is not good with my memory. But you're smart, so you would know that. What I don't know is if you woke him up at 2 AM and you said, HM, how's your memory? I don't know if he'd go, well, I'm a famous amnesic, of course it's terrible. I don't know if he knew it as a fact, like we might know about what we're good and not good at, or he just knew it by constantly being aware that he didn't know anything he ought to know via memory. Does that make sense? Anything else about--? Yeah. AUDIENCE: How does one explain scientific consent to--? PROFESSOR: Oh, this is good. It's a very interesting question. Informed consent. So technically he was conserved by the state once his family had passed away. But, you know, his intelligence was fine, so you had to be a little bit careful to do it right. But you could tell him, we're going to do an experiment. We're going to test your memory for words. Is that OK with you? And he'd say yes. Of course, if you had him read a 12-page document, by the time he got to page 12-- but you could talk him through it. His intelligence was fine. You could worry about the edges, if there-- so I think the investigators felt an extra ethical thing, also, not to ask him to do anything that a person might say no to. He was a wonderful participant, but it was an extra responsibility for the reason you're saying. AUDIENCE: Does that mean he was happy all the time? PROFESSOR: Was he happy all the time? Excellent question. He was a very sort of mellow-- yeah. Well, you could think for a moment whether having no memory would make you happy or sad. So he couldn't hold a job. He couldn't sustain a human relation because he would never know he'd met you before. So, those are things we often think of as pretty big in our happiness, the kind of the work we do, in some broad sense, and the people we relate to, right? And those were all gone for him. On the other hand, he didn't exactly realize that. He didn't realize he didn't work. He didn't realize he didn't have relations he might be expected to have, because he'd forget that about as fast as he could think about it. So I-- he was kind of a very, I would say, mild, happy person. So there's three possibilities that crossed my head. And scientifically, we can never figure them out. One of them is, maybe just he was that way. I mean some people are just mellow and happy, right? They don't have-- without amnesia. Second possibility is-- he had removed, also, the amygdala. We'll talk about that later in the course. That's a structure that's pretty important for some aspects of emotion and feeling. So that could have been relevant. The third one is, but this is kind of getting to your question, which was just-- intuitively, I feel like this. Again, this is not science but is just a feeling. That what makes us mostly sort of happy or sad? For me, it's mostly relatively recent things that went well or didn't go well, or things I'm sad about recently, and things on my horizon. Things I'm worried about, or things I'm looking forward to. And you guys might be different, but I don't get that worked up these days about second grade. At the time I'm sure it was pretty emotional about it. So if you think about him, he doesn't remember anything recent that was good or bad. He doesn't remember anything coming up that's interesting, or threatening, or risky, or unpleasant. So he lives in this constant now. You know, people tell you, live in the moment. Nobody could have lived more in the moment than he did. It's impossible to live more in the moment when the previous moment you can consult is from 30, 40 years ago. So, yeah, he was pretty happy, and a pretty pleasant guy. Essentially, I don't think most of us want that life, although he's probably happier than many people who have all their memories. One thing he's the opposite of, just to remind you-- we talked about it now, but just to make it clear-- it's the opposite of television amnesia. What happens in soap operas and murder movies or comedy shows when they get bonked on the head? Television amnesia. They forget who they are and where they came from, right? And then they marry their worst enemy or something like this, to make the story go forward. That's television amnesia. But after that, they're kind of fine, right? They're kind of operating but they just forgot where they come-- that we never see after brain injuries. We never see a person who forgets who they are but gets around fine in the world. HM is the opposite. He remembers stuff from before, but he can't learn new things. Opposite of TV amnesia. Any other questions about HM? Yeah. AUDIENCE: As he got older, could he recognize people from his past? AUDIENCE: Yes, and let me talk a little bit about that. If I don't answer that, let me know. So let me talk about this. This sort of touches on this, loss of already-known information. And if I don't answer you well, just put your hand up again, OK? So it's easy to test learning new things, because you can give somebody something in the laboratory and you see if they learn it. Knowing things they should have known from before is much harder. Different people know different things, in the past. So here's how they'd test it. And here's a thing that people have discovered, which is people are pretty unknowledgeable, mostly, about faces of people who were famous at the time they were famous. But if you're, like, 20, you don't know people who were famous before you were born. Mostly. There's some exceptions. So they'd take faces from different decades, and they'd show you them and they'd ask you, do you know who this is? So do you know who this is? This is Lindbergh. Frank Sinatra. Bob Hope. Does that name even ring a bell? I know. This is totally-- however old you are is totally how you do on this test. Lyndon Baines Johnson, president of the United States after Kennedy. Douglas MacArthur, the general. This, you might. Richard Nixon. McCarthy, as in McCarthyism. Golda Meir. Elvis. OK now. If you-- pretend you became amnesic. You became amnesic. Pretend you're old enough to become amnesic in 1990. This is if you're feeling-- would you know this face, in 1990? Let's say 1980. Let's make it simple. 1980? No. Would you know this face? No. OK. But you know them now. So that tells you that you can put faces to decades, roughly. You know, 1980, only Barack Obama's family knew Barack Obama's face, right? And now we all do. Temporally limited retrograde amnesia. So this is how well you do-- higher is better-- in decades. In 1960, here's HM. So here's faces that were famous in the '20s, '30s, and '40s, before he became amnesic, and he's pretty normal. These are public faces. And then he's terrible for the '50s and '60s, after his amnesia. So his memory for faces from the past is pretty normal. His memory for faces from the onset of his amnesia forward is terrible. Is that OK? Does that answer your question reasonably? But here's another really weird thing that people observed in many cases, which is kind of hard to understand, but it's been observed in many cases. So here's another patient. He was a bus driver in Washington, DC. He moved to work in a drugstore in Boston, then a mattress factory in Boston. Then he was hospitalized at the Boston VA in November 1965 with a huge hematoma on the right temporal parietal-- a big bleeding. And he was pretty aphasic and stuporous. He couldn't answer questions. In a month, digit span, his short-term memory returns. He states the date as approximately something like September 1965. He has a severe anterograde amnesia. For example, he fails to learned the names of the nurses he sees every day. But when asked-- this is the striking thing. When asked, where do you live? He says, I live in Washington, with certainty, as if he lost all these memories from before. By March, he starts to learn the nurses' names. He's recovering his memory capacity. He couldn't remember that he'd moved to Boston, but he doesn't fight the idea that he lives in Boston. Then he remembers that he worked at a drugstore. Then he remembers that he worked in a mattress factory. And by the time that he's discharged, it's only the last 24 hours before he went into the hospital that he's lost. So two things. There's a coupling between the anterograde amnesia and the retrograde amnesia. He can't learn new things, but he lost old things. And as the anterograde amnesia resolves, the retrograde amnesia resolves, in some sense, in temporal order. It's really odd, but it's been observed many times. So there is some link between what the hippocampus does and memory for the past. Now in some ways, it's really hard to study these things in detail in a patient like HM. But some other patients give us another perspective. Although every one of these examples is not quite perfect. Here's patients who undergo treatment for depression with electroconvulsive therapy. And appropriately-- in movies like One Flew Over the Cuckoo's Nest, wrongly used electroconvulsive therapy is wrong. For some patients with severe depression who fail to respond to medications and who have suicidal ideation-- literally attempt to take their lives or talk a lot about it in a believable way to family and physician-- electroconvulsive therapy can be very helpful. It's a hard choice to make, and a complicated one. Here's what happens. Patients typically go-- and it varies-- but they go in for the electroconvulsive therapy. And nowadays, it's done, typically, only in the right hemisphere. In this study, done some years ago around 1972, it was bilateral. They put electrodes on both sides of the brain. They passed current and they induced a seizure. And they do that something like every second day for about 10 days. It's a very aggressive form of treatment, certainly. But for some patients, the depression just lifts, and they're no longer talking about taking their lives. So for some people it seems to work when other things don't. During the course of the treatment, these patients develop and anterograde amnesia. They become HM-like. While they're in the hospital, they become HM-like. They can't learn new things. They forget the nurses. You can test them formally. And so Larry Squire did the following experiment with these kinds of patients. So we don't know that it's the hippocampus that's the critical thing in these patients, but they look a lot like HM. And we know that the hippocampus has a low seizure threshold. That's why it occurs so often as the locus of epilepsy. So what they did is they took television shows. And this is another world than you can possibly imagine. I grew up in a world where there were basically three television stations. And you're feeling sad for me already, right? And so everybody knew what was on those three television stations. But they took shows that weren't very successful, that were on for one season only. So they weren't super popular, but a lot of people saw them. And there were only three television networks and stations. So now what they did is they tested them on these events or other things like that before they had ECT-- so this is their knowledge before they had ECT. Of course they remember recent things, 1971, better than stuff from years ago. We all do. But the striking thing is that when these patients got ECT, during the course of their amnesia they only lost memory for TV shows from the last two years. They didn't lose those memories. These are the very same patients. The very same patients before they got ECT and while they're getting ECT, they lose memory only for the last two years of TV shows and not all the other ones. Or the same thing you can do with famous public events. So this is what people call a temporally-limited retrograde amnesia. It goes back for some years, the knowledge you already had. And here's the amazing thing. When these patients go home, their memory largely comes back and so does their knowledge of the TV shows. It's as if it's in their brain. They don't have access to it, for the last two years of information. But it's still sitting there and they regain that access. Two more examples on this. So here's an example-- because with humans it's always tough. So they do the same thing with monkeys. With monkeys, they could create a surgical lesion like HM and train them on material. They knew exactly what they learned. Here they had the surgery either 2, 4, 8 or 12, or 8, 16 weeks. And you can see the monkeys with the hippocampal damage were performing poorly if they learned the material 2 or 4 weeks ago but not 8 or 16 weeks ago. Again, a temporally-limited retrograde amnesia. So we're getting the idea that the hippocampus is important, not only for forming new memories-- and this is kind of wild. It's still important for you to remember stuff from a week ago, a month ago, and a couple years ago. And it takes years for your memory to become independent, or what people call consolidated, so that it no longer depends upon the hippocampus. So the hippocampus both is required to form a new memory, and it seems like it's necessary to remember that memory from months to years, before that memory becomes independent of the hippocampus. So right now in you, it's as if you had a neurochemistry team finishing your memories from the end of eighth grade. We're done. We're moving on to ninth grade! Because it takes years for your hippocampus to no longer be required to get those memories that you acquired years ago. And the same thing-- they did these famous faces, like the ones I showed you before? So these had to be somewhat older people. Here's activation in the 1990s, if they were looking at famous faces and the experiment was done in the late 1990s. So recent faces, hippocampus turned on, but not for '80s, '70s, or '60s, or '50s, or '40s. Again, they said the hippocampus is required to retrieve memories that's in the rest of your brain for some time period, but then after much time passes, it's no longer required. So this just summarizes those things. So now, Tyler, if we could do the film. We're going to how you an amnesic patient. Most amnesic patients are like this man. Very intelligent, very verbal. Not dramatic, but with terribly impaired memory. So last part of the talk is this idea of memory systems and procedural memory, the idea that you're a symphony of the neural systems that learn different things. So your brain is like the university, right? Department of Chemistry, department of Biology, department of Brain and Cognitive Science, or Economics. In you are multiple very specialized learning circuits that are good for learning different things. So this idea of a memory system, a particular part of the brain that has a particular learning process. And a key concept in that is, so far we've been talking about what people would normally call memory-- explicit or direct testing. You know, what did you study? What year is it? You're asked directly to do something. But another way to show memory and learning is through changes in performance with practice. So people will sometimes call the first kind declarative memory, where you directly have conscious memory about facts and episodes. You know that you know something, or that something occurred. And then the other kind, we'll talk about now, procedural memory, accessible only through performance and knowing how. Now with these kinds of skill learning or procedural memory, we see how people get better when they practice. Getting better is a change in your behavior that's learned and a change in your brain that performs. This is for your performance. So here's the experiment that was the original one Brenda Milner did with HM, where you have to trace a star in a mirror. You see your hand in a mirror, but you don't have direct view of your hand. Have any of you done something like this for some odds and ends reason? Anybody? Sometimes-- do I see a hand? No. OK. It's surprisingly hard. The horizontals and verticals are pretty easy. The diagonals, as you move your hand, you go, oh, it's no problem. I just reverse everything. And you see your hand just drift the wrong way. And you go, OK, no, no, no, no. I just have to go 45 degrees difference. I can do it. And you're controlling your hand, but then you move again and it still goes the wrong way. And so at first, you make lots of mistakes. We understand this because you have such automaticity between what you see and how you move all the time, that the mirror reverse, you have to overcome that automatic relationship. So at first you make a lot of mistakes and move slowly. You practice, and you get better. Here's the remarkable thing with-- so here's HM. Each of these dots is the next time he does it consecutively. He gets better from doing it over and over again. That's not a shock. Here's the shock. He comes the next day, he keeps the learning he had and he gets better. He comes in day three, he keeps learning. Now for him, a day is like infinity, for memory. When he comes in the room, he says, what do I do with this? You'd go, well, we did it the last two days. Don't you know? He said, no, I have no idea. Have you done this before? No. You can give him multiple choice of different things he might have done. He'll pick the wrong thing. When you come in on day three and you're this good, you're pretty proud. In fact, vain. You're going, like, I can do this. I'm awesome. Just let me at it. Because you know that you did it. You know how good you are through practice. He has no idea, but his learning is entirely normal. Not just better than you'd think? Just as good as you. So a memory system in his brain, different than the hippocampus, has kept that memory, uses that memory. And the amazing thing is that this means that that's also true in you, as far as we understand these things. When you learn a physical or mental skill, a lot of it is learned in a way that has nothing to do with your memory that you learned the skill. It's all by doing. And it doesn't depend on the hippocampus. And some years later, I did this again a little bit. Here's his performance first, second, third day, a week later, two weeks later, and a year later. Complete retention for a man who can forget within moments. A year later, a skill. He forgets that he did it ever within moments. So another kind of task that people do-- same idea. A motor skill you can lose, it's called rotary pursuit. There's a disc like this that revolves around a platter that turns pretty fast. Your job is to maintain contact between-- this was sort of sad before video games, right? This is about as interesting as 1940s games got. It was whizzed around, and it's fast enough that it's not easy to do. And at first your hand goes off it, but with practice you get really good at revolving with a rotating disc. And HM and patients like him learned that very well. Another motor skill. So who's bad at learning this? Which part of the brain is your instrument for learning skills through practice? So our best evidence comes from patients with a very difficult disease called Huntington's disease. It's a genetic rare disease. Its onset is in the 30s or 40s. These patients get severe motor problems and movement problems, and then, over time, cognitive ones and psychiatric ones. There's no treatment for it. So this is the caudate withering away in a patient with Huntington's disease compared to a healthy person's, post mortem. Or if you look at an MRI, here's your basal ganglia. Here's the withered away basal ganglia in the patients with Huntington's disease. And there's a brief-- can we do, Tyler-- there's a brief video of a patient with early-stage Huntington's disease. So it's not the most severe problem they have, but when they do one of these motor kinds of tasks-- here's normal typical people learning a motor task, getting better on it. It's good to be high on this graph. Here's the patients with Huntington's disease showing no learning at all. And this is a test that an amnesic patient would learn normally. So very convincing-- and you could say, well, if they have a motor problem, they're not going to get a motor skill. So one thing they do is they make the platter turn very slowly, so at the beginning they're doing just as well. And they still show no learning at all. So there's a lot of other evidence sparked by this that just shows that the basal ganglia is an instrument that's essential for us to learn physical or motor skills, perceptual skills, and a variety of cognitive skills as well. Things where we practice, practice, practice to become excellent-- basal ganglia is doing a lot of that work for you. The last kind of learning I'll talk to you about, and the last disease, is a sort of odder form of learning called repetition priming. It's a change in performance because of something you did recently. And if I give you a concrete example, it'll feel better. So here's one of many examples. Imagine you study a list of words like stamp, landmark, speak, clock. You study a list of words like these. I can give you an explicit or declarative memory test. What words did you see? You'd recall them. Or which word did you see, that we'd do multiple choice. That's regular memory that we know depends on the hippocampus. I can also give you weird test like this, which says, tell me the first word you think of that starts with STA. Now if you have good memory, you'll go, do you want me to give you stamp? Is that the point? And you go, no, no, no. And they go, oh, it's a Freudian thing. You want to know some weird things I'm gonna think of. And you go, no, no, no. Just tell me the first word you think of, OK? And you can give many answers that are all perfectly fine. You can give stall, stand, staccato, star, many STA words, and all of them are fine. That's all you have to do. But if you saw stamp, one particular completion of this, about 10 minutes ago, you're biased or primed to come up with that word. Not all the time, but much more than by chance. A recent experience biases you to behave in a certain way. And here's the remarkable thing. When you do this experiment with HM, if you just had the list of words and you wait about 20 seconds, here's your recall. Now you're not surprised that recall from a healthy person is about seven words, right? You tried that yourself. HM, sadly, has no score because he always goes, like, what list? So no recall. Multiple choice recognition, where there are three choices, here's typical people, HM about chance. The probability above chance-- that you give stamp as the completion to STA if you saw stamp-- if I showed you star, probably you'd give star. The specific word you saw 10 minutes ago? Perfectly the same in HM as normal control subjects. He was just as influenced by that word he saw 10 minutes ago as you are, to give you that answer. So this got people very excited, because they said, now we can experimentally and scientifically study something that Freud talked about as a very vague idea, the cognitive unconscious. He's not conscious that he saw a list of words 10 minutes ago, but it's making him choose to behave in a certain way now. Now you have both things in you, as far as we understand it. You can both remember you saw the word list, but if you switch your thinking a little bit, your behavior a little bit, the unconscious system gets into the driver's seat. Because the people with good memory were giving stamp often but no more or less than HM. So unconscious. So what part of the brain does this kind of memory? And our insight to that comes from Alzheimer's disease. So HM's kind of amnesia is extremely rare. Huntington's is quite rare. Alzheimer's is tragically common. In fact, they went to East Boston, the area near Logan, and knocked on doors of people's homes. Not people in nursing homes. At home. And they estimated-- and there's debates about this back and forth. But about half the people over 85 who answered the door qualify for diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. About half over 85. There's debates about this. There's debates about whether all of us would get it if we lived long enough. But as American society gets older, as us baby boomers all move into our '60s, '70s, and '80s, there's going to be a heck of a lot of people around with Alzheimer's disease. And some of you have probably experienced this, with grandparents if nothing else. It's a very devastating disorder for the patient, for the spouse or family. It's a very challenging disorder. So what is Alzheimer's disease? Well behaviorally, it's an insidious and progressive dementia. Dementia means, unlike HM, it's many losses of abilities. Unlike HM. HM had his surgery one day, boom. That was it. Alzheimer's starts very slowly and gets worse and worse over time. The biggest problem early on is memory, the most common problem but over time, language, thinking, concentration, spatial thinking, sometimes mood and personality, all of these get altered over time. In the brain when you look at the changes, you see that the changes occur especially in the hippocampal region-- so that's why the memory problem is like HM-- and also in some other parts of the cortex, but not so much, for example, in basal ganglia, that we just spoke about as important for procedural memory. Here's a control brain and equal-aged Alzheimer's brain. The reason why these gyri, this whole side widened is because of widespread neuronal shrinkage or death. And what's happening inside the brain, when you look at post mortem, there's sort of neurofibrillary tangles and plaques that are associated with the diseases. The tangles are something about the cells that have died. The plaques are thought to be maybe part of the process that leads to the disorder, but there's a debate about that to this day. And if you look, on post mortem, where the tangles are, the tangles up here, you can see they're most dense in the hippocampus or near the hippocampus, like HM. And so that's why we think so many patients with Alzheimer's have an HM-like memory problem and then more problems after that. So here's a healthy person top of the brain. Here's the left and right hippocampus in an 81-year-old. And here in an 80-year-old Alzheimer's patient, the hippocampus has withered away, both sides of the brain, bilaterally. So one more brain perspective on this. This is a SPECT scan, that just shows you where there's blood flow and metabolism. Top of the brain, bottom of the brain. Here's the insides of the temporal lobes in a healthy elderly adult. Look how much worse-- this is HM. He looks pretty good, right? You have to look really hard to see that the hippocampus is missing because it's such a small structure. And here's an early-stage Alzheimer's patient. Lots of the tissue is there, but a lot of-- you know, it's not functioning because there's so much damage. Here's the medial temporal lobes but also cortical areas that are compromised. OK, again. Not their biggest problem, but in terms of thinking about different parts of the brain and what parts of memory they do-- So these are amnesic patients, including HM, Alzheimer's patients, and green in controls. So recall of the list, what words were on the list, explicit memory bad in the two patient groups. Recognition pretty poor. Word stem completion, the kind of thing that we said is completely normal in HM, that's impaired in Alzheimer's disease. So we think that kind of priming depends on the neocortex, the part of the brain that's also injured in Alzheimer's. Because we know it doesn't depend on the hippocampus, because the patients with hippocampus-only damage are fine. So we end up with a picture like this of something on memory systems. For explicit or declarative memory, from patients with amnesia, we know it depends on the medial temporal lobe. Left, verbal; right, spatial. For skill learning, anything you practice many times to get better at, we know from Huntington's disease, that depends on the basal ganglia. For kinds of priming, recent experience altering something about brain organization, Alzheimer's disease, we know from that that it's likely mediated by the neocortex. These are from patient studies, but the imaging studies are very well aligned with these things. So we think it's true not only of patients, but of you, in terms of the instruments of your brain. So now we have not only episodic memory and semantic memory. We talked about procedural memory. We talked about priming. I didn't talk about conditioning today, but we've talked about that before. So I have two more slides to add on conceptually. So in what sense was Karl Ashley right or wrong when he said memory's all over the brain? Here's how we think about it now. We think he was right that all of your brain is learning. Practically all of your brain is plastic. It changes with experience. But different parts of your brain are learning different things. The basal ganglia is learning how to be skilled at something. The neocortex, how to gain knowledge and shift your representation of knowledge, like in priming. The medial temporal lobe on the left is learning verbal facts, and the right is nonverbal facts or spatial facts. So all of your brain is learning, but it's learning different things. And in that sense we end up with this metaphor that-- you know, just like there's a string section and a percussion section or something like that, in you, there's all these different instruments that are highly tuned in the way they're organized to learn all the different things you learned. They're all pretty useful instruments to have. And without these patients and the imaging studies, we wouldn't have known that. You would be like the Martian. One instrument? A thousand instruments? Well, we know there's multiple ones, and we know pretty much which structures are essential for them. Thanks.

Types

Types of RA can be divided into two main categories: temporally graded RA and pure forms of RA. Individuals with pure forms of RA like focal, isolated, and pure RA do not have anterograde amnesia (AA).

Temporally Graded RA

Memory loss in patients with temporally graded RA strongly follows Ribot's law, meaning that one will experience more memory loss for events closer to the injury or disease onset.[4] This type of RA is commonly triggered in individuals with Korsakoff syndrome due to a combination of long-term alcohol use and Wernicke encephalopathy.[7] Debate has risen about why this temporal gradient forms in the first place. Initial theories proposed that the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe are not nearly as important for long-term memories compared to short-term memories.[3] As memory processing occurs in the brain over time, neocortical regions can directly communicate with each other, so they do not rely as heavily on the hippocampus for long-term memory storage.[3] Therefore, if an individual experiences RA that damages the hippocampus, they will lose more short-term memories according to this theory. However, this theory has been challenged by the multiple-trace theory, which claims that the brain develops a hippocampal trace each time a memory is retrieved.[3] Since more hippocampal traces are present for older memories, it is easier for older memories to remain intact when RA occurs.[3]

Focal, Isolated, and Pure RA

An absence of anterograde amnesia (AA) characterizes pure forms of RA, which fall into three main categories: focal, isolated, and pure RA.[3] Slight differences in the use of these terms to describe a pure form of RA are summarized below:

Pure Forms of RA
Focal RA Isolated RA Pure RA
Focal RA generally results from neurological problems like epilepsy and is characterized by memory loss prior to – but not after – injury or disease onset.[8] When an individual experiences focal RA, a combination of their episodic and semantic memories may be affected.[9] Take a case study of a middle-aged female, for instance, who experienced focal RA after significant head trauma.[9] Although she could be re-taught information from her past, these memories were not episodic, but rather, semantic.[9] With focal RA, the details of a patient's life prior to amnesia onset can be reintroduced, but they are unable to recall how they perceived the experience.[9] Isolated RA is usually associated with a visible thalamic lesion.[10] Similar to other forms of RA, the inability to recall past information characterizes the isolated form.[10] Take the case of a middle-aged man identified as JG, whose thalamic lesion expanded as he grew older.[10] This lesion growth induced his isolated RA, resulting in both autobiographical memory loss and the inability to recognize information from popular culture.[10] Pure RA (PRA) is caused by a range of factors such as vascular diseases, encephalitis, and head injuries.[11] It is often confused with peritraumatic amnesia that commonly follows mild concussions, but the severity and duration of PRA differs from that of peritraumatic amnesia.[11] Current discussion in neuropsychiatry literature centers on whether PRA is possibly psychogenic in nature.[11]

Causes

RA commonly results from damage to regions of the brain that are associated with episodic and declarative memory, including autobiographical information. In extreme cases, individuals may completely forget who they are. Generally, this is a more severe type of amnesia known as global, or generalized amnesia.[12] However, memory loss can also be selective or categorical, manifested by a person's inability to remember events related to a specific incident or topic. Patients also differ in durations of RA (how long they can't recall information) and durations of what is forgotten (past time frame for which information is unavailable).

During consolidation, the hippocampus acts as an intermediate tool that quickly stores new information until it is transferred to the neocortex for the long-term. The temporal lobe, which holds the hippocampus, entorhinal, perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices, has a reciprocal connection with the neocortex.[13] The temporal lobe is temporarily needed when consolidating new information; as the learning becomes stronger, the neocortex becomes more independent of the temporal lobe.[13]

Studies on specific cases demonstrate how particular impaired areas of the hippocampus are associated with the severity of RA. Damage can be limited to the CA1 field of the hippocampus, causing very limited RA for about one to two years.[13] More extensive damage limited to the hippocampus causes temporally graded amnesia for 15 to 25 years.[13] Another study suggests that large medial temporal lobe lesions, that extend laterally to include other regions, produce more extensive RA, covering 40 to 50 years.[13] These findings suggest that density of RA becomes more severe and long-term as the damage extends beyond the hippocampus to surrounding structures.

Traumatic brain injury (TBI)

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) occurs from an external force that causes structural damage to the brain, such as a sharp blow to the head, a diffuse axonal injury,[14] or childhood brain damage (e.g., shaken baby syndrome).[14] In cases of sudden rapid acceleration, the brain continues moving around in the skull, harming brain tissue as it hits internal protrusions.[15]

TBI varies according to impact of external forces, location of structural damage, and severity of damage ranging from mild to severe.[16][14][15] Retrograde amnesia can be one of the many consequences of brain injury but it is important to note that it is not always the outcome of TBI. An example of a subgroup of people who are often exposed to TBI are individuals who are involved in high-contact sports. Research on football players takes a closer look at some of the implications to their high-contact activities. Enduring consistent head injuries can have an effect on the neural consolidation of memory.[17]

Specific cases, such as that of patient ML, support the evidence that severe blows to the head can cause the onset of RA.[18] In this specific case there was an onset of isolated RA following a severe head injury. The brain damage did not affect the person's ability to form new memories. Therefore, the idea that specific sections of retrograde memory are independent of anterograde is supported. Normally, there is a very gradual recovery, however, a dense period of amnesia immediately preceding the trauma usually persists.[17]

Traumatic events

RA can occur without any anatomical damage to the brain, lacking an observable neurobiological basis.[19] Primarily referred to as psychogenic amnesia or psychogenic fugue, it often occurs due to a traumatic situation that individuals wish to consciously or unconsciously avoid through intrapsychic conflicts or unconscious repressions.[20] The onset of psychogenic amnesia can be either global (i.e., individual forgets all history) or situation specific (i.e., individual is unable to retrieve memories of specific situations).[21]

Patients experiencing psychogenic amnesia have impaired episodic memory, instances of wandering and traveling, and acceptance of a new identity as a result of inaccessible memories pertaining to their previous identity.[20]

Recent research has begun to investigate the effects of stress and fear-inducing situations with the onset of RA. Long-term potentiation (LTP) is the process by which there is a signal transmission between neurons after the activation of a neuron, which has been known to play a strong role in the hippocampus in learning and memory.[21] Common changes in the hippocampus have been found to be related to stress and induced LTP.[21] The commonalities support the idea that variations of stress can play a role in producing new memories as well as the onset of RA for other memories.[21] The amygdala plays a crucial role in memory and can be affected by emotional stimuli, evoking RA.[22]

Studies of specific cases, such as 'AMN', support evidence of traumatic experiences as a plausible cause of RA. AMN escaped a small fire in his house, did not inhale any smoke, and had no brain damage. Nevertheless, he was unable to recall autobiographical knowledge the next day. This case shows that RA can occur in the absence of structural brain damage.[23]

After a traumatic head injury, emotional disturbances can occur at three different levels: neurological, reactionary, and long-term disturbances. Neurological disturbances can change emotional and motivational responses. Reactionary disturbances effect emotional and motivational responses as well, but reflect the failure to cope with environmental demands. Someone with this might withdraw from the environment that they are placed in because they no longer know how to handle the cognitive resources.[24]

Nutritional deficiency

RA has been found among alcohol-dependent patients who have Korsakoff's syndrome.[25] Korsakoff's syndrome patients develop retrograde amnesia due to a thiamine deficiency (lack of vitamin B1).[26] Also, chronic alcohol use disorders are associated with a decrease in the volume of the left and right hippocampus.[25]

These patients' regular diet consists mostly of hard alcohol intake, which lacks the necessary nutrients for typical development and maintenance.[26] Therefore, after a prolonged time consuming primarily alcohol, these people undergo memory difficulties and ultimately develop RA. However, some of the drawbacks of using Korsakoff patients to study RA is the progressive nature of the illness and the unknown time of onset.[13]

Infections

Infections that pass the blood–brain barrier can cause brain damage (encephalitis), sometimes resulting in the onset of RA. In the case of patient 'SS', the infection led to focal or isolated retrograde amnesia where there was an absence of or limited AA. Brain scans show abnormalities in the bilateral medial temporal lobes, including two thirds of the hippocampal formation and the posterior part of the amygdala.[27]

Surgery

Henry Molaison had epilepsy that progressed and worsened by his late twenties. The severity of his condition caused him to undergo surgery in an effort to prevent his seizures. Unfortunately, due to a lack of overall known neurological knowledge, Molaison's surgeons removed his bilateral medial temporal lobe, causing profound AA and RA.[28] The removed brain structures included the hippocampus, the amygdala, and the parahippocampal gyrus, now called the medial temporal lobe memory system.[28] HM was one of the most studied memory cases to date and started the examination of neurological structures in relation to memory.

Patients who have RA due to surgery are "P.B." and "F.C." who had unilateral removal of the medial areas in the left temporal lobe.[28]

Controlled induction

Clinically induced RA has been achieved using different forms of electrical induction.

  • Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), used as a depression therapy, can cause impairments in memory.[29] Tests show that information from days and weeks before the ECT can be permanently lost.[30] The results of this study also show that severity of RA is more extreme in cases of bilateral ECT rather than unilateral ECT. Impairments can also be more intense if ECT is administered repetitively (sine wave simulation) as opposed to a single pulse (brief-pulse stimulation).[31]
  • Electroconvulsive shock (ECS): The research in this field has been advanced by using animals as subjects.[32] This is done to further understand RA.

Diagnosis

Since RA affects people's memories to varying degrees, testing is required to fully diagnose RA; these tests, however, are inherently limited if a patient's previous neuropathological medical history is unknown.[33] As a result, some clinicians diagnose RA by testing patients about factual knowledge, such as current public events.[33] This testing is limited, however, because people's knowledge about current events differs.[33] Furthermore, these tests must be adjusted to account for the time period that a patient is alive, which affects the amount of detail included in the questions.[34] Since some information obtained from this testing is subjective, it is difficult to verify how accurately memories are recalled; this difficulty is especially true for memories from the distant past.[33]

To avoid these issues, many researchers test for RA using the Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI).[33][35] The AMI asks patients targeted questions about three different portions of their life: childhood, early adult life, and recent life.[33] For each period of that individual's life, researchers ask questions that require the patient to use either their autobiographical or semantic memory.[35] Through the AMI, researchers can better understand the types of memories affected, as well as the degree of a patient's RA.[33] These AMIs can then be used alongside functional brain imaging techniques like magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), computed tomography scans (CT) and electroencephalography (EEG) that detect brain damage in patients with RA.[6]

Brain structures

The most commonly affected areas are associated with episodic and declarative memory such as the hippocampus,[36] the diencephalon,[37] and the temporal lobes.[38]

  • The hippocampus deals largely with memory consolidation,[36] where information from the working memory and short-term memory is encoded into long-term storage for future retrieval. Amnesic patients with damage to the hippocampus are able to demonstrate some degree of unimpaired semantic memory, despite a loss of episodic memory, due to spared parahippocampal cortex.[39] In other words, retrograde amnesics "know" about information or skill, but cannot "remember" how they do.
  • The diencephalon and the surrounding areas' role in memory is not well understood. However, this structure appears to be involved in episodic memory recall.[37]
  • The temporal lobes are essential for semantic and factual memory processing. Aside from helping to consolidate memory with the hippocampus,[38] the temporal lobes are extremely important for semantic memory. Damage to this region of the brain can result in the impaired organization and categorization of verbal material, disturbance of language comprehension, and impaired long-term memory. The right frontal lobe is critical for the retrieval of episodic information, while the left frontal region is more active for the retrieval of semantic information.[40] Lesions in the right hemisphere and right frontal lobes result in the impaired recall of non-verbal material, such as music and drawings.[41] Difficulties in studying this region of the brain extends to its duties in comprehension, naming objects, verbal memory, and other language functions.[42]

Brain plasticity has helped explain the recovery process of brain damage induced retrograde amnesia, where neuro-structures use different neural pathways to avoid the damaged areas while still performing their tasks.[43] Thus, the brain can learn to be independent of the impaired hippocampus, but only to a certain extent.[13] For example, older memories are consolidated over time and in various structures of the brain, including Wernicke's area and the neocortex, making retrieval through alternate pathways possible.[4]

Case studies

Since researchers are interested in examining the effects of disrupted brain areas and conducting experiments for further understanding of an unaffected, normal brain,[44] many individuals with brain damage have volunteered to undergo countless tests to advance our scientific knowledge of the human brain. For example, Henry Molaison (HM) was someone with significant brain damage and participated in a lot of neurological research. Furthermore, he was also the most tested person in neuropsychology.[45] All living people who participate are referred to in literature using only their initials to protect privacy.

Each case of RA has led to different symptoms and durations, where some patients have exhibited an inability to describe future plans, whether in the near future (e.g., this afternoon) or in the distant future (e.g., next summer)[46] because of their inability to consolidate memories.[13] Furthermore, researchers have also found that some patients can identify themselves and loved ones in photographs, but cannot determine the time or place the photo was taken.[46] It has also been found that patients with RA greatly differ from the general population in remembering past events.[47]

A few case examples are:

  • After a head injury, AB had to relearn personal information.[48] Many of AB's habits had also changed.[48]
  • Patient CD reported disorientation of place and time following his injuries as well as relearning previously learned information and activities (e.g., using a razor).[48]
  • EF was examined and found to be very confused about social norms (e.g., appropriate attire outside his home). EF exhibited memory loss of his personal experiences (e.g., childhood), and the impaired ability to recognize his wife and parents.[48]
  • JG is the first recorded patient with isolated RA.[10]
  • GH, a mother and a wife, had surgery in August 2002. When GH woke up after the surgery, she believed it was May 1989.[48] Due to her amnesia, GH experienced great difficulty in her social environment, being overwhelmed by relationships to others.[48]
  • DH, a learning disabilities instructor and husband, sustained a closed head injury. He did not show any normal signs of memory loss but he could not recall anything prior to the accident.[1]
  • CDA is a 20-year-old man who fell and experienced head trauma after being unconscious for a little less than an hour. He had a self-identity loss and a retrograde deficit limited to the autobiographical events 5 years before the trauma. He often showed signs of spontaneous speech that was iterative and sometimes incoherent. When he saw his family and friends, he was shocked at how old they looked because he remembered them from 5 years earlier. This case also included amnesia for procedural skills like the fear of shaving or driving, which ultimately was overcome. There were no psychological, neuropsychological, or brain damage problems. His recovery of memory was progressive and spontaneous, where after several months the amnesia was limited to the two years preceding the trauma. This was a classic case of PRA.[11]
  • GC was a 38 year old accountant that was found in a town square unable to remember anything about himself and unaware of where he was and how he got there. He was eventually able to recall basic information about himself and his family, but could not recall emotionally charged autobiographical events pertaining to the last 7 years of his life. Within 3–4 days, it was determined that his autobiographical amnesia was clearly and strictly selective for professional events, as he could remember everything that was not related to his job. It was ultimately learned that the job had created severe emotional stress and anxiety due to the extreme hours that triggered a sudden fugue state. He was eventually able to recover most of his memories minus a single work event where he had stolen money from the company. This was a classic case of psychogenic amnesia.[11]
  • AF is a 15-year-old boy who hit his head and lost consciousness. He could not remember anything but was able to play songs on the piano, showing that his procedural memory was still intact. He gradually recovered some memories within the first 2–3 days but had autobiographical amnesia as well as significant memory loss for famous public facts and events for the 2 years prior to the injury.[11]
  • L is 19-year-old student who was left with the inability to recall episodic memories after experiencing a fugue state in December of 2020. However, he was able to recall things such as his birthday and the street names of Nantes, the city where he resides in. L was part of a case study that associated reduced pupil size as a possible indicator of RA.[49]

Although it may seem that people living with brain damage have great difficulty continuing the usual day-to-day aspects, they still can accomplish many feats. People with RA are able to lead a normal life. For instance, KC is a man who has many functional aspects intact; normal intelligence, unaffected perceptual and linguistic skills, short-term memory, social skills, and reasoning abilities.[46] All of these things are necessary in everyday life and contribute to normal living. KC also is fully capable of scripted activities (e.g., making reservations or changing a flat tire).[46] In addition, patient HC successfully graduated high school and continued into post-secondary studies,[47] an obvious accomplishment despite her condition. DH relearned his childhood memories from his parents and can retell the stories, but cannot recall specifics other than what has been told to him.[1]

Other forms of amnesia

Other forms of amnesia exist and may be confused with RA. For instance, anterograde amnesia (AA) is the inability to learn new information.[50] This describes a problem encoding, storing, or retrieving information that can be used in the future.[51] It is important to note that these two conditions can, and often do both occur in the same patient simultaneously,[16] but are otherwise separate forms of amnesia.

RA can also be an inherent aspect of other forms of amnesia, namely transient global amnesia (TGA). TGA is the sudden onset of AA and RA caused by a traumatic event, however it is short lived, typically lasting only 4 to 8 hours[52] TGA is very difficult to study because of the patients' quick recovery.[53] This form of amnesia, like AA, remains distinct from RA.[13]

Post-traumatic amnesia (PTA) is a state of confusion that occurs immediately following a traumatic brain injury in which the injured person is disoriented and unable to remember events that occur after the injury.

Psychogenic amnesia, or dissociative amnesia, is a memory disorder characterized by sudden retrograde autobiographical memory loss, said to occur for a period of time ranging from hours to years.

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c Hunkin NM, Parkin AJ, Bradley VA, Burrows EH, Aldrich FK, Jansari A, Burdon-Cooper C (April 1995). "Focal retrograde amnesia following closed head injury: a case study and theoretical account". Neuropsychologia. 33 (4): 509–523. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(94)00136-D. PMID 7617158. S2CID 21295568.
  2. ^ Lafleche G, Verfaellie M (2017). "Anterograde Amnesia". In Kreutzer J, DeLuca J, Caplan B (eds.). Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Cham: Springer International Publishing. pp. 1–5. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-56782-2_1106-2. ISBN 978-3-319-56782-2.
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h i Lafleche G, Verfaellie M (2011). "Retrograde Amnesia". In Kreutzer J, DeLuca J, Caplan B (eds.). Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. New York, NY: Springer New York. pp. 2167–2170. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_1152. ISBN 978-0-387-79947-6.
  4. ^ a b c Wixted JT (February 2004). "The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting". Annual Review of Psychology. 55: 235–269. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555. PMID 14744216. S2CID 3057114.
  5. ^ Reed JM, Squire LR (May 1998). "Retrograde amnesia for facts and events: findings from four new cases". The Journal of Neuroscience. 18 (10): 3943–54. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03943.1998. PMC 6793126. PMID 9570821.
  6. ^ a b Medical Advisory, Secretariat (2006-12-01). "Functional Brain Imaging". Ontario Health Technology Assessment Series. 6 (22): 1–79. ISSN 1915-7398. PMC 3379170. PMID 23074493.
  7. ^ Bauer RM, Asken B (2008). "Chapter 28: The Three Amnesias". In Morgan JE, Ricker JH (eds.). Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Rutlidge. pp. 678–700. doi:10.4324/9781315537511-51. ISBN 978-1-351-98599-4.
  8. ^ Sehm B, Frisch S, Thöne-Otto A, Horstmann A, Villringer A, Obrig H (2011-10-19). de Beeck HP (ed.). "Focal retrograde amnesia: voxel-based morphometry findings in a case without MRI lesions". PLOS ONE. 6 (10): e26538. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...626538S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026538. PMC 3197527. PMID 22028902.
  9. ^ a b c d Wheeler MA, McMillan CT (March 2001). "Focal retrograde amnesia and the episodic-semantic distinction". Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 1 (1): 22–36. doi:10.3758/CABN.1.1.22. PMID 12467101. S2CID 20269516.
  10. ^ a b c d e Miller LA, Caine D, Harding A, Thompson EJ, Large M, Watson JD (2001). "Right medial thalamic lesion causes isolated retrograde amnesia". Neuropsychologia. 39 (10): 1037–46. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00041-0. PMID 11440756. S2CID 18021762.
  11. ^ a b c d e f Lucchelli F, Muggia S, Spinnler H (1998). "The Syndrome of Pure Retrograde Amnesia". Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 3 (2): 91–118. doi:10.1080/135468098396189.
  12. ^ Vaidya CJ, Gabrieli JD, Verfaellie M, Fleischman D, Askari N (April 1998). "Font-specific priming following global amnesia and occipital lobe damage". Neuropsychology. 12 (2): 183–92. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.12.2.183. PMID 9556765.
  13. ^ a b c d e f g h i Squire LR, Alvarez P (April 1995). "Retrograde amnesia and memory consolidation: a neurobiological perspective". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 5 (2): 169–177. doi:10.1016/0959-4388(95)80023-9. PMID 7620304. S2CID 9080102.
  14. ^ a b c Wolf JA, Stys PK, Lusardi T, Meaney D, Smith DH (March 2001). "Traumatic axonal injury induces calcium influx modulated by tetrodotoxin-sensitive sodium channels". The Journal of Neuroscience. 21 (6): 1923–30. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-06-01923.2001. PMC 6762603. PMID 11245677.
  15. ^ a b Hardman JM, Manoukian A (May 2002). "Pathology of head trauma". Neuroimaging Clinics of North America. 12 (2): 175–87, vii. doi:10.1016/s1052-5149(02)00009-6. PMID 12391630.
  16. ^ a b Winocur G, McDonald RM, Moscovitch M (2001). "Anterograde and retrograde amnesia in rats with large hippocampal lesions". Hippocampus. 11 (1): 18–26. doi:10.1002/1098-1063(2001)11:1<18::AID-HIPO1016>3.0.CO;2-5. PMID 11261769. S2CID 4079756.
  17. ^ a b Yarnell PR, Lynch S (April 1970). "Retrograde memory immediately after concussion". Lancet. 1 (7652): 863–864. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(70)91691-0. PMID 4191508.
  18. ^ Levine B, Black SE, Cabeza R, Sinden M, Mcintosh AR, Toth JP, et al. (October 1998). "Episodic memory and the self in a case of isolated retrograde amnesia". Brain. 121 (Pt 10): 1951–1973. doi:10.1093/brain/121.10.1951. PMID 9798749.
  19. ^ Staniloiu A, Markowitsch HJ, Brand M (September 2010). "Psychogenic amnesia--a malady of the constricted self". Consciousness and Cognition. 19 (3): 778–801. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.024. PMID 20655759. S2CID 19950039.
  20. ^ a b Markowitsch HJ (November 2003). "Psychogenic amnesia". NeuroImage. 20 (Suppl 1): S132–S138. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.010. PMID 14597306. S2CID 18671395.
  21. ^ a b c d Diamond DM, Park CR, Woodson JC (2004). "Stress generates emotional memories and retrograde amnesia by inducing an endogenous form of hippocampal LTP". Hippocampus. 14 (3): 281–91. doi:10.1002/hipo.10186. PMID 15132427. S2CID 45819426.
  22. ^ Strange BA, Hurlemann R, Dolan RJ (November 2003). "An emotion-induced retrograde amnesia in humans is amygdala- and beta-adrenergic-dependent". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 100 (23): 13626–13631. doi:10.1073/pnas.1635116100. PMC 263864. PMID 14595032.
  23. ^ Markowitsch HJ, Kessler J, Van Der Ven C, Weber-Luxenburger G, Albers M, Heiss WD (January 1998). "Psychic trauma causing grossly reduced brain metabolism and cognitive deterioration". Neuropsychologia. 36 (1): 77–82. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00093-6. PMID 9533390. S2CID 19174159.
  24. ^ Prigatano GP (1987). "Psychiatric aspects of head injury: Problem areas and suggested guidelines for research". Neurobehavioral Recovery from Head Injury. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 217–218.
  25. ^ a b De Bellis MD, Clark DB, Beers SR, Soloff PH, Boring AM, Hall J, Kersh A, Keshavan MS (May 2000). "Hippocampal volume in adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 157 (5): 737–44. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.737. PMID 10784466. S2CID 1668456.
  26. ^ a b McEntee WJ, Mair RG (August 1990). "The Korsakoff syndrome: a neurochemical perspective". Trends in Neurosciences. 13 (8): 340–4. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(90)90146-2. PMID 1699320. S2CID 31404753.
  27. ^ Fujii T, Yamadori A, Endo K, Suzuki K, Fukatsu R (December 1999). "Disproportionate retrograde amnesia in a patient with herpes simplex encephalitis". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 35 (5): 599–614. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70822-0. PMID 10656630. S2CID 4480119.
  28. ^ a b c Squire LR (January 2009). "The legacy of patient H.M. for neuroscience". Neuron. 61 (1): 6–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.023. PMC 2649674. PMID 19146808. S2CID 643599.
  29. ^ Ottosson J (1960). "Experimental studies of the mode of action of electroconvulsive therapy". Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 145: 1–41. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1960.tb08352.x. S2CID 29773758.
  30. ^ Donahue AB (June 2000). "Electroconvulsive therapy and memory loss: a personal journey". The Journal of ECT. 16 (2): 133–43. doi:10.1097/00124509-200006000-00005. PMID 10868323. S2CID 28318459.
  31. ^ Squire LR (1986). "Memory functions as affected by electroconvulsive therapy". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 462 (1): 307–314. Bibcode:1986NYASA.462..307S. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1986.tb51265.x. PMID 3458411. S2CID 28060335.
  32. ^ Rao SK, Andrade C, Reddy K, Madappa KN, Thyagarajan S, Chandra S (May 2002). "Memory protective effect of indomethacin against electroconvulsive shock-induced retrograde amnesia in rats". Biological Psychiatry. 51 (9): 770–773. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01219-7. PMID 11983192. S2CID 29560202.
  33. ^ a b c d e f g Reed JM, Squire LR (May 1998). "Retrograde amnesia for facts and events: findings from four new cases". The Journal of Neuroscience. 18 (10): 3943–54. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-10-03943.1998. PMC 6793126. PMID 9570821.
  34. ^ Baddeley A, Eysenck MW, Anderson MC (2015). Memory. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 444–48.
  35. ^ a b Kopelman, M. D.; Wilson, B. A.; Baddeley, A. D. (1989). "The autobiographical memory interview: A new assessment of autobiographical and personal semantic memory in amnesic patients". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 11 (5): 724–744. doi:10.1080/01688638908400928. ISSN 0168-8634. PMID 2808661.
  36. ^ a b Cipolotti L, Shallice T, Chan D, Fox N, Scahill R, Harrison G, Stevens J, Rudge P (2001). "Long-term retrograde amnesia...the crucial role of the hippocampus". Neuropsychologia. 39 (2): 151–72. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00103-2. PMID 11163373. S2CID 7428309.
  37. ^ a b Dusoir H, Kapur N, Byrnes DP, McKinstry S, Hoare RD (December 1990). "The role of diencephalic pathology in human memory disorder. Evidence from a penetrating paranasal brain injury". Brain. 113 (Pt 6): 1695–1706. doi:10.1093/brain/113.6.1695. PMID 2276041.
  38. ^ a b Buccione I, Fadda L, Serra L, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA (November 2008). "Retrograde episodic and semantic memory impairment correlates with side of temporal lobe damage". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 14 (6): 1083–94. doi:10.1017/S1355617708080922. PMID 18954490. S2CID 12905302.
  39. ^ Nadel L, Moscovitch M (April 1997). "Memory consolidation, retrograde amnesia and the hippocampal complex". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 7 (2): 217–227. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80010-4. PMID 9142752. S2CID 4802179.
  40. ^ Reinvang I, Nielsen CS, Gjerstad L, Bakke SJ (September 2000). "Isolated retrograde amnesia: Evidence for preservation of implicit memory. An event-related potential investigation". Neurocase. 6 (5): 423–433. doi:10.1080/13554790008402713. S2CID 145665210.
  41. ^ Milner B (1968). "Visual recognition and recall after right temporal lobe excision in man". Neuropsychologia. 6 (3): 191–209. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(68)90019-5.
  42. ^ Kolb B, Whishaw I (1990). Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology (3rd ed.). New York: Freeman. ISBN 978-0-7167-1973-1.[page needed]
  43. ^ Kolb B, Gibb R, Robinson TE (February 2003). "Brain plasticity and behavior". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 12 (1): 1–5. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01210. hdl:2027.42/74427. S2CID 11271886.
  44. ^ Esclapez M, Tillakaratne NJ, Kaufman DL, Tobin AJ, Houser CR (March 1994). "Comparative localization of two forms of glutamic acid decarboxylase and their mRNAs in rat brain supports the concept of functional differences between the forms". The Journal of Neuroscience. 14 (3 Pt 2): 1834–1855. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-03-01834.1994. PMC 6577546. PMID 8126575.
  45. ^ Corkin S, Amaral DG, González RG, Johnson KA, Hyman BT (May 1997). "H. M.'s medial temporal lobe lesion: findings from magnetic resonance imaging". The Journal of Neuroscience. 17 (10): 3964–79. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-10-03964.1997. PMC 6573687. PMID 9133414.
  46. ^ a b c d Tulving E, Schacter DL, McLachlan DR, Moscovitch M (August 1988). "Priming of semantic autobiographical knowledge: a case study of retrograde amnesia". Brain and Cognition. 8 (1): 3–20. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(88)90035-8. PMID 3166816. S2CID 17125951.
  47. ^ a b Kwan D, Carson N, Addis DR, Rosenbaum RS (September 2010). "Deficits in past remembering extend to future imagining in a case of developmental amnesia". Neuropsychologia. 48 (11): 3179–3186. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.011. PMID 20561535. S2CID 565582.
  48. ^ a b c d e f Fujiwara E, Brand M, Kracht L, Kessler J, Diebel A, Netz J, Markowitsch HJ (January 2008). "Functional retrograde amnesia: a multiple case study". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior. 44 (1): 29–45. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2005.09.001. PMID 18387529. S2CID 206982684.
  49. ^ El Haj, Mohamad; Lamy, Estelle; Janssen, Steve M. J.; Boutoleau-Bretonnière, Claire (2021-03-04). "Amnesia in your pupils: decreased pupil size during autobiographical retrieval in a case of retrograde amnesia". Neurocase. 27 (2): 155–159. doi:10.1080/13554794.2021.1902539. ISSN 1355-4794. PMID 33739239. S2CID 232299779.
  50. ^ Dworetzky BA (2001). "The neurology of memory". Seminars in Speech and Language. 22 (2): 95–105. doi:10.1055/s-2001-13934. PMID 11373070. S2CID 25906826.
  51. ^ Markowitsch HJ, Calabrese P (1999). "Neuroanatomy of memory". In Tulving E, Craik FI (eds.). The Oxford Handbook of Memory. Oxford University Press. pp. 465–484.
  52. ^ Fisher CM (October 1982). "Transient global amnesia. Precipitating activities and other observations". Archives of Neurology. 39 (10): 605–8. doi:10.1001/archneur.1982.00510220003001. PMID 7125972.
  53. ^ Guillery-Girard B, Desgranges B, Urban C, Piolino P, de la Sayette V, Eustache F (November 2004). "The dynamic time course of memory recovery in transient global amnesia". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 75 (11): 1532–1540. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2003.024968. PMC 1738827. PMID 15489382. S2CID 16685800.
  54. ^ Woodruff B, Patria M (19 April 2010). "Man With Amnesia Lost 46 Years in Workplace Slip: Scott Bolzan Interviews Wife for Details on Their 25-Plus Year Marriage, Past Life". Nightline. ABC News.

External links

This page was last edited on 11 March 2024, at 08:46
Basis of this page is in Wikipedia. Text is available under the CC BY-SA 3.0 Unported License. Non-text media are available under their specified licenses. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc. WIKI 2 is an independent company and has no affiliation with Wikimedia Foundation.