To install click the Add extension button. That's it.

The source code for the WIKI 2 extension is being checked by specialists of the Mozilla Foundation, Google, and Apple. You could also do it yourself at any point in time.

4,5
Kelly Slayton
Congratulations on this excellent venture… what a great idea!
Alexander Grigorievskiy
I use WIKI 2 every day and almost forgot how the original Wikipedia looks like.
Live Statistics
English Articles
Improved in 24 Hours
Added in 24 Hours
Languages
Recent
Show all languages
What we do. Every page goes through several hundred of perfecting techniques; in live mode. Quite the same Wikipedia. Just better.
.
Leo
Newton
Brights
Milds

Monothematic delusion

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Monothematic delusion
SpecialtyPsychiatry

A monothematic delusion is a delusional state that concerns only one particular topic. This is contrasted by what is sometimes called multi-thematic or polythematic delusions where the person has a range of delusions (typically the case of schizophrenia). These disorders can occur within the context of schizophrenia or dementia or they can occur without any other signs of mental illness. When these disorders are found outside the context of mental illness, they are often caused by organic dysfunction as a result of traumatic brain injury, stroke, or neurological illness.

People who experience these delusions as a result of organic dysfunction often do not have any obvious intellectual deficiency nor do they have any other symptoms. Additionally, a few of these people even have some awareness that their beliefs are bizarre, yet they cannot be persuaded that their beliefs are false.[citation needed]

YouTube Encyclopedic

  • 1/3
    Views:
    1 837
    1 718
    415
  • Project PERFECT 2015: Lisa Bortolotti
  • delusional perception
  • Project PERFECT 2015: Ema Sullivan-Bissett

Transcription

Types

Some delusions that fall under this category are:

  • Capgras delusion: the belief that (usually) a close relative or spouse has been replaced by an identical-looking impostor.
  • Fregoli delusion: the belief that various people whom the believer meets are actually the same person in disguise.
  • Intermetamorphosis: the belief that people in one's environment swap identities with each other while maintaining the same appearance.
  • Subjective doubles: a person believes there is a doppelgänger or double of themselves carrying out independent actions.
  • Cotard delusion: the belief that oneself is dead or does not exist; sometimes coupled with the belief that one is putrefying or missing internal organs.
  • Mirrored-self misidentification: the belief that one's reflection in a mirror is some other person.
  • Reduplicative paramnesia: the belief that a familiar person, place, object, or body part has been duplicated. For example, a person may believe that they are, in fact, not in the hospital to which they were admitted, but in an identical-looking hospital in a different part of the country.
  • Somatoparaphrenia: the delusion where one denies ownership of a limb or an entire side of one's body (often connected with stroke).

Note that some of these delusions are sometimes grouped under the umbrella term of delusional misidentification syndrome.

Causes

Current cognitive neuropsychology research points toward a two-factor approach to the cause of monothematic delusions.[1] The first factor being the anomalous experience—often a neurological defect—which leads to the delusion, and the second factor being an impairment of the belief formation cognitive process.

As an example of one of these first factors, several studies point toward Capgras delusion being the result of a disorder of the affect component of face perception. As a result, while the person can recognize their spouse (or other close relation) they do not feel the typical emotional reaction, and thus the spouse does not seem like the person they once knew.

As studies have shown, these neurological defects are not enough on their own to cause delusional thinking.[citation needed] An additional second factor—a bias or impairment of the belief formation cognitive process—is likely required to solidify and maintain the delusion. This need for a second factor results in two factor theories to explain monothematic delusions—anomalous experience and some sort of underlying pathology of belief production. Since we do not currently have a solid cognitive model of the belief formation process, this second factor is still somewhat of an unknown, and some hypotheses regarding delusion formation don't posit a second factor as necessary.[2]

Some research has shown that delusional people are more prone to jumping to conclusions,[3][4][5] and thus they would be more likely to take their anomalous experience as veridical and make snap judgments based on these experiences. Additionally, studies[5] have shown that they are more prone to making errors due to matching bias—indicative of a tendency to try and confirm the rule. These two judgment biases help explain how delusion-prone people could grasp onto extreme delusions and be very resistant to change.

Researchers claim this is enough to explain the delusional thinking. However, other researchers still argue that these biases are not enough to explain why they remain completely impervious to evidence over time. They believe that there must be some additional unknown neurological defect in the patient's belief system (probably in the right hemisphere[6]).

See also

References

  1. ^ Davies, M., Coltheart, M., Langdon, R., Breen, N. (2001). "Monothematic delusions: Towards a two-factor account" (PDF). Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology. 8 (2): 133–158. doi:10.1353/ppp.2001.0007. S2CID 43914021. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-03-02.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  2. ^ Noordhof, Paul; Sullivan-Bissett, Ema (2021-06-21). "The clinical significance of anomalous experience in the explanation of monothematic delusions". Synthese. Springer Science and Business Media LLC. 199 (3–4): 10277–10309. doi:10.1007/s11229-021-03245-x. ISSN 0039-7857. S2CID 237820651.
  3. ^ Sellen, J., Oaksford, M., Langdon, R., Gray, N. (2005). "Schizotypy and Conditional Reasoning". Schizophrenia Bulletin. 31 (1): 105–116. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbi012. PMID 15888430.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  4. ^ Dudley RE, John CH, Young AW, Over DE (May 1997). "Normal and abnormal reasoning in people with delusions". British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 36 (Pt 2): 243–58. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1997.tb01410.x. PMID 9167864.
  5. ^ a b Stone, T. (2005). "Delusions and Belief Formation" (Powerpoint).[dead link]
  6. ^ Gurin, Lindsey; Blum, Sonja (2017). "Delusions and the Right Hemisphere: A Review of the Case for the Right Hemisphere as a Mediator of Reality-Based Belief". The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. American Psychiatric Association Publishing. 29 (3): 225–235. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16060118. ISSN 0895-0172. PMID 28347214.

External links

  • The Belief Formation Project a project of the Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science, which uses research on delusions with the aim of developing a cognitive model of beliefs (link accessed on February 1, 2016)
This page was last edited on 19 July 2023, at 20:48
Basis of this page is in Wikipedia. Text is available under the CC BY-SA 3.0 Unported License. Non-text media are available under their specified licenses. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc. WIKI 2 is an independent company and has no affiliation with Wikimedia Foundation.