To install click the Add extension button. That's it.

The source code for the WIKI 2 extension is being checked by specialists of the Mozilla Foundation, Google, and Apple. You could also do it yourself at any point in time.

4,5
Kelly Slayton
Congratulations on this excellent venture… what a great idea!
Alexander Grigorievskiy
I use WIKI 2 every day and almost forgot how the original Wikipedia looks like.
Live Statistics
English Articles
Improved in 24 Hours
Added in 24 Hours
Languages
Recent
Show all languages
What we do. Every page goes through several hundred of perfecting techniques; in live mode. Quite the same Wikipedia. Just better.
.
Leo
Newton
Brights
Milds

Functional Ensemble of Temperament

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Functional Ensemble of Temperament (FET) is a neurochemical model suggesting specific functional roles of main neurotransmitter systems in the regulation of behaviour.

YouTube Encyclopedic

  • 1/3
    Views:
    20 336
    4 002
    8 611
  • The difference between adults and grown ups: Dr. Lisa Damour at TEDxCLE
  • Charles Rosen & Daniel J. Wakin Inaugurate 21st-Century Music in Society at the Graduate Center
  • Baroque Musical Style - OpenBUCS

Transcription

Hi. So everything that I know about what it means to be a grown-up, I've learned from teenagers. I'm a psychologist who works with teenagers and doing this work has taught me so much about growing up. And when I say growing up. I don't just mean aging into adulthood, lots of people do that. I mean maturing into a real grown-up. Now, all of us are connected to teenagers whether it's our teenager or somebody else's. So I'm gonna share what I've learned about the differences between people who are merely adults and people who are really grown-ups, and I'm gonna do this to clarify how all of us fit in to the process of helping young people grow up. Now I will tell some stories from my work with teenagers, and I won't share the details of any one person's life, but I'll share what are really amalgams of many moments I've spent with teenagers over the years. Alright, so let's get down to business. So the first difference that I've observed between people who are adults, so not really grown up, and people who have really grown up has to do with risk assessment. So, what I mean is, how do we decide what chances to take? The way I see it, is that people who are merely adults, who haven't really grown up, assess risk in terms of the chances of getting caught engaging in risky behavior. (Laughter) In contrast, people who are really grown up assess risk in terms of the chances in term of the actual consequences of the behaviors that they're considering. So using this as an example we see signs like this all the time. So people who are not grown-up see a sign like this and think, "Yeah. But what are the chances there's a cop around the corner. People who are grown-ups see signs like this and think, "Well, of course you have to slow down to take a curve. you can't safely take a curve going full speed." Now the issue of risk assessment is especially critical when we talk about teenagers. If we look at data like this about risk taking over the life span, you see this big peak in adolescence. The fact of the matter is – teenagers take more chances than they should. And as a result, they do have higher accident rates than almost any other age group. So, here's a story from my work that really clarified for me this whole issue about risk assessment. So, it's a Thursday afternoon and a 17-year-old girl comes into my office and she's in a great mood and she says, "Oh gosh! I can't wait to tell you about my plans for the weekend." So the plans for the weekend are that she's going to have a sleepover on a friend's boat. And she's going to have this sleepover with a boy who she does not know well. And – the kicker, right? She's going to do all of this without her mother knowing where she is. (Laughter) So she's telling me the story, but she says, "It's okay. I've thought it all through. I've thought it all through." And she goes on to describe what she's thought through and all that she has thought through is how she's not gonna get caught by her mother, right? So she's telling me this and of course it involves she's telling her mom she'll be at a sleepover at a friend's house, and that friend knows to call my client, should the mother call that house. And I'm listening to this, right, I'm supposed to be coming up with something useful to say, But I'm listening and all I am thinking is, "I'm gonna call your mom, right?" (Laughter) "You walk out that door, I'm on the phone with your mother, right?" But, okay. So here's the problem. So tattling on teenagers to their parents is not my job, right? Actually, protecting their confidentiality and helping them turn into grown-ups is my job. So, I pull myself together and I say the word, "OK, look. You and I both know that you getting caught by your mom is the least dangerous thing that could possibly happen to you this weekend. And I don't know that she knew it, but I always give benefit of the doubt and it worked. It got our conversation going in the right direction. And we started talking about the actual risks that she would be facing with this incredibly dangerous plan. And luckily, she just decided to cancel the plans. So, now if you're a parent and you're thinking, "OK, well, how do I get my teenager to do this? How do I get my young person to start to think in more mature ways?" I think you've got some say. So if your teenager says things to you like, "Hey, what would you do if you caught me texting while driving?" I think we should resist our first impulse to say things like, "I would ground you until you were 45! I would take away your phone." And I think instead, we could say things like, "I would be so glad I caught you, before you hurt or killed yourself or somebody else." So in another words, when we frame the consequences in terms of getting caught I think we actually give teenagers the wrong message. When we frame the consequences in terms of the actual dangers they would be facing. I think we start to help them to move towards being grown-ups. OK, here's my second one. Crazy Spots. This is what I call crazy spots at any rate. So I kind of have bad news for everybody on this one. We all have crazy spots. And what I mean, when I say crazy spots, is that we all have aspects of our personality that are totally irrational. And that the people around us – the people around us should not take it personally. OK? So now what does this have to do with adults and grown-ups? Well, people who are really grown-ups have actually learned and accepted that their parents have crazy spots, OK? Som in other words, they've learned and accepted that their parents have aspects of their personalities that are not well suited to parenting. And that do not need to be taken personally. And this is tough, because I think even though we all can kind of know that parents are just people who had kids, right? And even though we can all kind of know – We all kind of know that all parents come with limitations. We know this and yet we kind of wish that's just gonna be true for everybody else's parents, right? Not for my parents. And when we come up against ours parents' limitations, I think we take it personally. So a huge step in growing up is to stop taking personally everything your parents do. So when teenagers come into my work, and we're doing our work together and they complain to me about their parents which, not surprisingly, is some of what we spend time talking about. I never question their complaints, I never ask them about the details of it. Instead, I say things like, "OK, now I hear that this bugs you about your mom, but what do you think this is all about for her? I try to get them to see it from a new perspective. I try to get them to move from the kind of egocentric childlike view of the world that they start with to a view where they can see their parent as having a free-standing personality. A personality that was in place long before that child was born and a personality that's gonna be in place long after that child has moved out. Now, the capacity to do this, the capacity to think in these ways doesn't even become available until adolescence. So 70 years ago, Jean Piaget the developmental psychologist talked about the emergence of what he called "formal operations", which is the ability to do abstract reasoning, to think about thinking. What's amazing is that modern neuroscience has actually supported all of what he described in this area. We used to think the brain stopped developing by age age 10 or 11, but what we know now is that there's incredible development in the brain all the way through adolescence. And you see it especially appear in the area that's in a red, which is the frontal lobe, which is where all of the very sophisticated thinking goes on. So, this sophisticated thinking makes for amazing things that can happen. So here, I think about a 15-year-old girl I worked with who really struggled with her dad around money. She would complain to me that he would do things like require her to use her own money to buy school clothes, and he would say to her, "I don't have the money for it." And then a couple of days later he would drive home with some really nice new car. So she was hurt by this, she was confused by this. It bothered her a lot. She didn't understand, "Why can he be generous with himself, but not with me?" And then, about a week after Thanksgiving she comes into my office and she says – she tells this amazing story. I love the way teenagers talk. She says,"So, Oh my god! So, you know – My grandma came for Thanksgiving. My dad's mom comes for Thanksgiving. And while she was at our house, I could overhear her talking to my dad. And it was clear from the conversation that she had promised to give him a check for his birthday, but that she was changing her mind and that she was gonna keep the money. And I could hear my dad talking to her, and it was clear that he really wanted the money, but he didn't want have to beg his mother for the money. And I'm thinking, Oh my god! That's why dad's so weird about money! Because grandma is so weird about money. (Laughter) Alright? So, I love them. I love them. And here's what's amazing. When a teenager makes this step, two incredible things happen. First, all this energy is freed up. All of the energy that this young woman had been expending on fighting with her dad, trying to change her dad, feeling hurt by her dad, trying to make sense of him she could let it all go in some much more profitable direction. The other thing, and this is the part I love the most, actually. They become more tender towards their parents, and they become more tender in the exact same domain where they had previously been so annoyed with the parent. So, this girl still did feel hurt by her dad at times about money, but she could also at the same time feel some sympathy for him because she knew that for him this was actually an area where he really struggled. So, I wanna put a little couple of caveats in here. One is, when it comes to coming to terms with parents' crazy spots, I think for most of us this starts in adolescence, but continues for many many years after that. (Laughter) I think the other, and this is very much, you know – I think all people who know people would agree is that – if the parents' limitations are extensive, if the parent has lots and lots and lots of crazy spots coming to terms with them is not so easy. So, this is a bit of a plug for psychology and psychotherapy. (Laughter) OK, so, you may be listening to this and thinking, "Hmm. This is pretty interesting, but how does it fit in for parents?" So, here's how I want you to think about it. If you're a parent and you want to try to promote this growth in your child, you could own your crazy spots, right? I know this is a very tall order. But if you're the father I just described, you might say something like, "Hey, I got bad news and good news for you. The bad news is: I do not handle money well. This is not an area that is very comfortable for me. The good news is: this is my issue not yours. This is not something you need to be taking personally. You have to work with it, you have to work around it, but you don't need to take it personally. Now if you're thinking, "Oh! That's so funny, you know? I really don't have any crazy spots. (Laughter) I'm thinking about it, and I just don't. (Laughter) OK, well I got bad news and good news for you, right? So the bad news is: you do have crazy spots. Everybody has crazy spots. The good news is: if you want to know what they are, just ask the people who love you, right? (Laughter) You can ask your children, you can ask your partner, you can ask your friends, they'll let you know. OK. So finally, another difference between people who are adults, merely adults, and people who are really grown-ups has to do with what I call goal orientation. We all know people who don't have goal orientation. They kind of drift from one thing to the next. They may even make a plan, but as soon as something comes up, or gets in the way, they give it up. In contrast, people who are grown-ups make and pursue meaningful goals. They set aside short term gratifications, they can deal with obstacles in order to get some bigger payoff down the line. Now, we think about it, "OK, that sounds great. This is what we want for teenagers. How do we get them there?" This is an area where teenagers benefit tremendously from advice and guidance from grown-ups around them. I think about a young man I worked with who was actually really getting himself into trouble. He was messing around with drugs, he had started skipping classes in high school. He was really at risk of not making it out of high school. Somehow, in the midst of this, he got himself a job as a busboy at a small restaurant and the owner of the restaurant liked him, the owner of the restaurant made a connection with him. And when a spot opened up in the kitchen he offered it to the guy. The young man started working in the kitchen and he just fell in love with cooking. And it was clear from what I was hearing of the story, that the owner of the restaurant was definitely cultivating the romance between this young man and cooking. And the young man decided he wanted to go to culinary school. At which point, he started going to class because he knew he couldn't go to culinary school without a high school diploma. So in the end, he actually did become a chef. But even if he hadn't, even if he had changed his mind, he would've changed his mind with a diploma in hand, which makes all the difference. So, now you may be a parent listening to this, thinking, "Yeah, that sounds great. I would love to give advice and guidance to my teenager. She won't do anything I say, right?" And this is true. There's something about being a teenager, something during puberty, that seems to happen where teenagers get this reflex installed in them that if their parents suggests it, it is completely off the table. You know, and we think about – we all have seen this happening like the teenage boy who was walking towards the coat closet on a cold day, but if his mom says, "You should probably bundle up!" He will turn around and leave the house on purpose without his coat on. Now, evolutionary psychologists say this is a good thing. They say this is good because for children to reject the advice of their parents has probably contributed to all of this innovation in the culture over the years. I know this is a cold comfort for parents, but at the same time if we think about it like, "OK, who discovered fire, right? I am so sure it was a cave teenager messing around. (Laughter) So where does this leave us? Well, luckily teenagers don't reject advice from everybody, mostly just from their parents. In fact, every teenager I know wants to be taken seriously, wants to be treated like somebody who's going somewhere. And I think we can all think of somebody who did this for us, right? Somebody who looked past our youth and maybe our goofiness and communicated in one way or another, "I see that you have a contribution to make, I see that you are headed somewhere." So what this means is that we absolutely must surround teenagers with devoted teachers, and coaches, and bosses, and family friends, and aunts and uncles. We need more – fewer people who say, "Ugh. Teenagers." And more people who understand that it is the work of the entire community to help young people grow up. So I'll leave you with this. Working with teenagers can actually be pretty stressful. And I think largely it's because being a teenager can be very very stressful. I do know some teenagers who basically, function like grown-ups, but most teenagers are still making their way. Because there is so much at stake in this point in development. We really have so much of a better handle if we do more understanding and less handwringing about teenagers. I think that my hope is that in stepping back from the swirl of adolescent development and breaking down the exact kind of growth we do want to see in these years. My hope is that it has helped to orient all of us to how we fit in to this process of helping young people grow up and not just age into adulthood. Thank you. (Applause)

Earlier theories

Medications can adjust the release of brain neurotransmitters in cases of depression, anxiety disorder, schizophrenia and other mental disorders because an imbalance within neurotransmitter systems can emerge as consistent characteristics in behaviour compromising people's lives. All people have a weaker form of such imbalance in at least one of such neurotransmitter systems that make each of us distinct from one another. The impact of this weak imbalance in neurochemistry can be seen in the consistent features of behaviour in healthy people (temperament). In this sense temperament (as neuro-chemically-based individual differences) and mental illness represents varying degrees along the same continuum of neurotransmitter imbalance in neurophysiological systems of behavioural regulation.[1][2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11]

In fact, multiple temperament traits (such as Impulsivity, sensation seeking, neuroticism, endurance, plasticity, sociability or extraversion) have been linked to brain neurotransmitters and hormone systems.[12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19]

By the end of the 20th century, it became clear that the human brain operates with more than a dozen neurotransmitters and a large number of neuropeptides and hormones. The relationships between these different chemical systems are complex as some of them suppress and some of them induce each other's release during neuronal exchanges. This complexity of relationships devalues the old approach of assigning "inhibitory vs. excitatory" roles to neurotransmitters: the same neurotransmitters can be either inhibitory or excitatory depending on what system they interact with. It became clear that an impressive diversity of neurotransmitters and their receptors is necessary to meet a wide range of behavioural situations, but the links between temperament traits and specific neurotransmitters are still a matter of research. Several attempts were made to assign specific (single) neurotransmitters to specific (single) traits. For example, dopamine was proposed to be a neurotransmitter of the trait of Extraversion, noradrenaline was linked to anxiety, and serotonin was thought to be a neurotransmitter of an inhibition system. These assignments of neurotransmitter functions appeared to be an oversimplification when confronted by the evidence of much more diverse functionality.[16][17] Research groups led by Petra Netter in Germany, Lars Farde in Karolinska Institute in Sweden and Trevor Robbins in Cambridge, UK had most extensive studies of the links between temperament/personality traits or dynamical properties of behavior and groups of neurotransmitters.[20][21][22][23][24][25]

Development of the FET model

The architecture of the Functional Ensemble of Temperament (FET) was developed by Trofimova as the Compact version of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ-77) in 1997–2007. The differentiation between the rows of the FET inherits the activity-specific approach to the structure of temperament proposed by Rusalov in mid-1980s. According to this approach, the traits of temperament (and behavioural regulation) related to motor-physical, social-verbal and mental aspects of activities are based on different neurophysiological systems and should be assessed separately (so you can see a separation of traits into 3 rows related to these 3 types of activities). The 3-column structure of the FET framework follows Alexander Luria theory of three functional neuroanatomic systems (sensory-informational, programming and energetic) and is in line with functional constructivism approach.[26] This approach views all behaviour as being constructed and generated anew based on individual's capacities and demands of the situation [19]

The final STQ-77/FET model considers 12 systems (and temperament traits): 9 systems (and traits) regulating the formal functional aspects of behaviour (energetic, dynamic and orientational, each assessed in three domains (intellectual, physical and social-verbal) together with 3 systems related to emotionality (Neuroticism, Impulsivity and a disposition of Satisfaction (formerly called Self-Confidence) (see Figure).[27][28][29][30][31][32][19][33]

The differences between Trofimova's and Rusalov's models of temperament (and the structures of their versions of the STQ) are:

  • the choice of the grouping of temperament traits by three dynamical aspects (endurance, speed of integration of actions and orientation), presented as three columns in the Figure;
  • a presence of orientation-related traits in Trofimova's model which were not included in Rusalov's model. These traits describe the behavioural orientation of a person with preferences to specific types of reinforcers: sensations (Sensation Seeking), other people's state (Empathy)[34] or knowledge about causes of natural processes (a trait named as Sensitivity to Probabilities).[32]
  • a different structure of the traits related to emotionality. FET considers emotionality traits as systems amplifying three dynamical aspects of behaviour presented in the three columns of the model. Amplification of orientation aspects emerges in the trait of Neuroticism; amplification of speed of integration (i.e. immature integration) emerges as Impulsivity and amplification of subjective feeling of energetic capacities emerges in the trait of Satisfaction (Self-confidence).[34]
The structure of neurochemical framework Functional Ensemble of Temperament (FET) corresponds to 12 universal aspects of construction of behaviour. Shadowed text relates to temperament traits; blue capital letters highlight the lead, and grey – supporting neurotransmitters for the given trait. Note: 5-HT: serotonin; DA: dopamine; NE: noradrenalin; ACh: acetylcholine; Glu: Glutamate; H: histamine; A: adenosine; Estr: estrogen; NP: neuropeptides; GH: Growth Hormone; ORE: orexins, KOR, MOR, DOR: kappa-, delta- and mu-opioid receptors systems correspondingly; HPA: hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis; Tstr – Testosterone, Adr – Adrenaline, Cort – Cortisol

.

In 2007–2013 this STQ-77 model of temperament was reviewed and compared to the main findings in neurophysiology, neurochemistry, clinical psychology and kinesiology resulting in the neurochemical FET model offered by Irina Trofimova, McMaster University.[16] Trevor Robbins, Cambridge University who collaborated with Trofimova on this project in 2014–2016 suggested a revision of the part of the FET neurochemical hypothesis related to the trait of Intellectual (mental) Endurance (known in cognitive psychology also as "sustained attention"). This neurochemical component of the FET hypothesis was upgraded in 2015 by underlying a key role of acetylcholine and noradrenalin in sustained attention.[16][17][35] In February 2018, by the suggestion of Dr Marina Kolbeneva (Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences) the scale Self-Confidence was renamed as the scale of dispositional Satisfaction.[19]

Overview of the model

The final STQ-77/FET framework classifies temperament traits and their neurochemical biomarkers into 12 components: nine components regulating the formal functional aspects of behaviour (energetic, dynamic and orientational) each assessed in three domains (intellectual, physical and social-verbal); also three components related to emotionality (Neuroticism, Impulsivity and Satisfaction (Self-Confidence)) (see Figure) [19][34][33][36]

The FET framework summarized existing literature showing the nine non-emotionality traits are regulated by the monoamines (MA) (noradrenalin, dopamine, serotonin), acetylcholine and neuropeptide systems, whereas the three emotionality-related traits emerge as a dysregulation of opioid receptors systems that have direct control over MA systems. Importantly, the FET model suggests that there is no one-to-one correspondence between the neurotransmitter systems underlying temperament traits (or mental disorders) but instead specific ensemble relationships between these systems emerge as temperament traits.[16][17][31][32][19] The FET framework is based only on the strongest consensus points in the research studying the role of neurotransmitter in behavioural regulation and the components of temperament; it doesn't list more controversial links between these multiple systems.

Temperament trait Description Hypothesized links to neurochemical systems
Behavioral orientation traits NE +...
Sensation seeking behavioral orientation to well-defined and existing sensational objects and events, underestimation of outcomes of risky behaviour high Testosterone, fluctuating Adrenaline (leading to occasional HPA under-arousal), low cortisol, in interactions with the brain NE systems
Empathy behavioral orientation to the emotional states/needs of others versus Self-orientation (ranging from social dependency/semi-delusional social perception to selfishness and insensitivity to social expectations known in Asperger syndrome) Higher OXY and lower vasopressin functions interacting with the brain NE system
Sensitivity to Probabilities (analytic abilities) the drive to gather information about commonality, frequency and values of events, to differentiate their specific features, to project these features in future actions Interaction between neocortical Glu, NE, DA and ACh systems
Speed of action-integration traits DA +...
Physical Tempo speed of integration of an action in physical manipulations with objects with well-defined scripts of actions DA-GABA interaction in basal ganglia, DOR, with involvement of ACh interneurons, NP and adenosine
Social-verbal Tempo the preferred speed of speech and ability to understand fast speech on well-known topics, reading and sorting of known verbal material Estrogen, NP under DA control, especially in dorsal striatum
Plasticity the ability to adapt quickly to changes in situations, to generate new program of action, and to shift between different tasks DA-ACh-GABA interaction with ACh systems in the cortex-basal ganglia networks
Maintenance of activity traits 5-HT, ACh +...
Physical Endurance the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged physical activity using well-defined behavioral elements 5-HT, orexins, NP, histamine
Social-verbal Endurance (sociability) sociability; the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged social-verbal activities using well-defined behavioral elements 5-HT, estrogen, histamine
Mental Endurance (sustained attention) the ability to stay focused on selected features of objects with suppression of behavioral reactivity to other features Neocortical NE-ACh (with a lead of ACh for tonic attention and a lead of NE for novelty aspects), supported by cortical 5-HT
Emotional amplifier traits Opioid receptor systems + ...
Neuroticism A tendency to avoid novelty, unpredictable situations and uncertainty. KOR, cytokins or some Gut microbiota leading to activation of HPA and/or NE release
Impulsivity Initiation of actions based on immediate emotional reactivity sANS, HPA axis → DOR > MOR, DA
Satisfaction A sense of security, disposition for a good mood; in high values – overconfidence with negligence to details MOR, some Gut microbiota →(5-HT, DA)

Neurotransmitter systems: 5-HT: serotonin; DA: dopamine; NE: noradrenalin; ACh: acetylcholine; Glu: glutamate; OXY: oxytocin; VSP: vasopressin; NP: Neuropeptides; KOR, MOR, DOR: kappa-, mu- and delta-opioid receptors correspondingly; sANS - sympathetic autonomic nervous system; HPA - hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis.

The FET points out that opioid receptor systems are involved not only in regulation of emotional dispositions but also amplify three non-emotionality aspects of behaviour (KOR for orientation, DOR for integration of actions and MOR of approval-maintenance of behaviour).[31][19] This involvement was confirmed for MOR systems that bind endorphins: experiments show that MOR overstimulation influences hypothalamic serotonin and Brain-derived neurotrophic factor release and affecting endurance aspects of behaviour.[37][38][39] The interplay within hormonal systems and its interaction with serotonin also appeared to be a factor is social emotions, such as shame and guilt [40]

FET model and classifications of psychiatric disorders

FET framework was proposed to simplify classifications of psychiatric disorders (DSM, ICD) using 12 functional aspects of behaviour that this model highlights.[41][11][19] Clinical studies showed good differential power of the FET framework for various diagnoses of psychopathology. For example, depressed people had low endurance and psychomotor slowdown in their temperament profiles.[9][10][11][41] In contrast to them, patients with Generalized Anxiety Disorder had higher impulsivity and neuroticism.[9][8][11] FET-developers suggested that every symptom in DSM/ICD diagnoses can be mapped into a specific FET code reflecting a disregulation within well-documented neurochemical systems.[19]

References

  1. ^ Clark, L.A., Watson, D., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality, and mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(1), 103–116.
  2. ^ Cloninger CR (ed). Personality and psychopathology. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press, 1999.
  3. ^ Mehrabian, A. (1995). Distinguishing depression and trait anxiety in terms of basic dimensions of temperament. Imagination. Cognition and Personality, 15(2), 133-143.
  4. ^ Ball, S.A., Tennen, H., Poling, J.C., Kranzlen, H.R., & Rounsaville, B.J. (1999). Personality, temperament, and character dimensions and DSM-IV personality disorders in substance abusers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(4), 545-553
  5. ^ Brown, T.A. (2007). Temporal course and structural relationships among dimensions of temperament and DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorder constructs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(2), 313-328
  6. ^ Karam, E.G., Salamoun, M.M., Yeretzian, J.S., Neimneh, Z.N., Karam, A.N. et al. (2010). The role of anxious and hyperthymic temperaments in mental disorders: a national epidemiologic study. World Psychiatry, 9(2), 103–110.
  7. ^ Watson, D. & Naragon-Gainey, K. (2014). Personality, emotions, and emotional disorders. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(4), 422-442.
  8. ^ a b Trofimova, IN; Christiansen, J. (2016). "Coupling of temperament traits with mental illness in four age groups". Psychological Reports. 118 (2): 387–412. doi:10.1177/0033294116639430. PMID 27154370. S2CID 24465522.
  9. ^ a b c Trofimova, IN; Sulis, W (2016). "Benefits of distinguishing between physical and social-verbal aspects of behaviour: an example of generalized anxiety". Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 338. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00338. PMC 4789559. PMID 27014146.
  10. ^ a b Trofimova, IN; Sulis, W (2016). "A study of the coupling of FET temperament traits with major depression". Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 1848. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01848. PMC 5123189. PMID 27933018.
  11. ^ a b c d Trofimova, I.; Sulis, W. (2018). "There is more to mental illness than negative affect: Comprehensive temperament profiles in depression and generalized anxiety". BMC Psychiatry. 18 (1): 125. doi:10.1186/s12888-018-1695-x. PMC 5946468. PMID 29747614.
  12. ^ Gray, J.A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press
  13. ^ Netter, P. (1991) Biochemical variables in the study of temperament. In Strelau, J. & Angleitner, A. (Eds.), Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on theory and measurement 147-161. New York: Plenum Press.
  14. ^ Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:975-990.
  15. ^ Depue, R.A., & Morrone-Strupinsky, J.V. (2005). A neurobehavioural model of affiliate bonding: implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Journal of Behavioural and Brain Science, 28(3), 313-350.
  16. ^ a b c d e Trofimova, IN (2016). "The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of adult temperament". In: Arnold, M.C. (Ed.) Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.: 77–147.
  17. ^ a b c d Trofimova, IN; Robbins, TW (2016). "Temperament and arousal systems: a new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 64: 382–402. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.008. hdl:11375/26202. PMID 26969100. S2CID 13937324.
  18. ^ Depue, R. & Fu, Y. (2012) Neurobiology and neurochemistry of temperament in adults. In: Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. (Eds.) Handbook of Temperament. NY: Guilford Publications, 368-399. (2012).
  19. ^ a b c d e f g h i Trofimova, I. (2021). "Contingent tunes of neurochemical ensembles in the norm and pathology: can we see the patterns?". Neuropsychobiology. 80 (2): 101–133. doi:10.1159/000513688. PMID 33721867. S2CID 232243254.
  20. ^ Netter, P. (1991) Biochemical variables in the study of temperament. In Strelau, J. & Angleitner, A. (Eds.) Explorations in temperament: international perspectives on theory and measurement, 147-161. New York:Plenum Press.
  21. ^ Netter, P. (2006) Dopamine challenge tests as an indicator of psychological traits. Human psychopharmacology: clinical and experimental 21:91–99.
  22. ^ Netter, P., Hennig, J. & Roed, I. (1996) Serotonin and dopamine as mediators of sensation seeking behaviour. Neuropsychobiology 34:155–65.
  23. ^ Robbins, T.W., & Roberts, A.C. (2007) Differential regulation of fronto-executive function by the monoamines and acetylcholine. Cerebral Cortex 17 (Suppl 1):151–160.
  24. ^ Walker, S.C., Robbins, T.W., & Roberts, A.C. (2009) Differential contributions of dopamine and serotonin to orbitofrontal cortex function in the marmoset. Cerebral Cortex 19(4):889-898.
  25. ^ Robbins, T.W., & Everitt, B.J. (1996) Arousal Systems and Attention. In: Gazzaniga, M. The Cognitive Neurosciences 703-720. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  26. ^ Trofimova, I. (2017). "Functional Constructivism: In Search of Formal Descriptors". Nonlinear Dynamics in Psychology and Life Sciences. 21 (4): 441–474. PMID 28923156.
  27. ^ Rusalov, VM; Trofimova, IN (2007). Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Services Press.
  28. ^ Trofimova, IN (2010). "Questioning the "general arousal" models". Open Behavioral Science and Psychology. 4: 1–8. doi:10.2174/1874230001004010001.
  29. ^ Trofimova, IN (2010). "An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality". American Journal of Psychology. 123 (4): 467–480. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467. PMID 21291163.
  30. ^ Trofimova, IN; Sulis, W (2011). "Is temperament activity-specific? Validation of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire – Compact (STQ-77)". International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy. 11 (3): 389–400.
  31. ^ a b c Trofimova, IN (2018). "Functionality vs dimensionality in psychological taxonomies, and a puzzle of emotional valence". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 373, 1744 (1744). doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0167. PMC 5832691. PMID 29483351.
  32. ^ a b c Trofimova, IN (2019). "An overlap between mental abilities and temperament traits". McFarland, D. (Ed.) General and Specific Mental Abilities. Cambridge Scholars Publishing, Cambridge, UK: 176–225.
  33. ^ a b Trofimova, I (2021). "Functional constructivism approach to multilevel nature of biobehavioural diversity". Frontiers in Psychiatry. 12: 641286. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2021.641286. PMC 8578849. PMID 34777031.
  34. ^ a b c Trofimova, I; Gaykalova, A (2021). "Emotionality vs. other biobehavioural traits: a look at neurochemical biomarkers for their differentiation". Frontiers in Psychology. 12: 781631. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.781631. PMC 8720768. PMID 34987450.
  35. ^ Robbins, TW (2018). "Opinion on monoaminergic contributions to traits and temperament". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 383 (1744). doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0153. PMC 5832679. PMID 29483339.
  36. ^ Trofimova, I (2022). "Transient nature of stable behavioural patterns, and how we can respect it". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 44: 641286. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101109. S2CID 247249864.
  37. ^ Yoshioka M, Matsumoto M, Togashi H, Smith CB, Saito H (1993). "Opioid receptor regulation of 5-hydroxytryptamine release from the rat hippocampus measured by in vivo microdialysis". Brain Research. 613 (1): 74–79. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(93)90456-w. hdl:2027.42/30746. PMID 8394180. S2CID 1556815.
  38. ^ Fujita W, Gomes I, Devi LA (2014). "ORevolution in GPCR signalling: opioid receptor heteromers as novel therapeutic targets: IUPHAR review 10". Br J Pharmacol. 171 (18): 4155–4176. doi:10.1111/bph.12798. PMC 4241085. PMID 24916280. S2CID 9840181.
  39. ^ Rezaei S, Bakhshani NM, Fanaei H, Trofimova I (2021). "Opium effect in pregnancy on the dynamics of maternal behaviour: testing a neurochemical model". Neuropsychobiology. 80 (2): 147–157. doi:10.1159/000512698. ISSN 0302-282X. PMID 33611320. S2CID 231987701.
  40. ^ Kanen JW, Robbins TW, Trofimova IN (2021). "Harnessing Temperament to Elucidate the Complexities of Serotonin Function". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 45: 101108. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101108. S2CID 247297736.
  41. ^ a b Sulis, W (2018). "Assessing the continuum between temperament and affective illness: psychiatric and mathematical perspectives". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373 (1744). doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0168. PMC 5832692. PMID 29483352.

External links

This page was last edited on 2 December 2023, at 22:12
Basis of this page is in Wikipedia. Text is available under the CC BY-SA 3.0 Unported License. Non-text media are available under their specified licenses. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc. WIKI 2 is an independent company and has no affiliation with Wikimedia Foundation.